Back to Journals » Psychology Research and Behavior Management » Volume 17
A Longitudinal Investigation on the Reciprocal Relationship of Problematic Smartphone Use with Bedtime Procrastination, Sleep Quality, and Mental Health Among University Students
Authors Cemei L, Sriram S, Holý O, Rehman S
Received 4 April 2024
Accepted for publication 24 July 2024
Published 28 September 2024 Volume 2024:17 Pages 3355—3367
DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S472299
Checked for plagiarism Yes
Review by Single anonymous peer review
Peer reviewer comments 2
Editor who approved publication: Professor Mei-Chun Cheung
Li Cemei,1 Shyamkumar Sriram,2 Ondřej Holý,3 Shazia Rehman4– 6
1Faculty of Education, University Malaya, Kuala Lumpur, 50603, Malaysia; 2Department of Social and Public Health, Ohio University, Athens, OH, 45701, USA; 3Science and Research Centre, Faculty of Health Sciences, Palacký University Olomouc, Olomouc, Czechia; 4Department of Psychiatry, National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders and National Center for Mental Disorders, The Second Xiangya Hospital of Central South University, Changsha, Hunan, 410011, People’s Republic of China; 5Mental Health Institute of Central South University, China National Technology Institute on Mental Disorders, Hunan Technology Institute of Psychiatry, Hunan Key Laboratory of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Hunan Medical Center for Mental Health, Changsha, Hunan, 410011, People’s Republic of China; 6Department of Psychology, Quaid-e-Azam University, Islamabad, Pakistan
Correspondence: Shazia Rehman, Department of Psychiatry, National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders and National Center for Mental Disorders, The Second Xiangya Hospital of Central South University, Changsha, Hunan, 410011, People’s Republic of China, Tel +86-13308429613, Email [email protected]
Background: Problematic smartphone use (PSU) is linked to various mental health issues, but the relationship between PSU, bedtime procrastination, and mental health symptoms is unclear. Sleep factors related to PSU and its mental health effects have been understudied. This study explores the longitudinal associations between PSU, bedtime procrastination, sleep quality, and mental health in university students.
Methods: In this study, a total of 683 university students participated by completing questionnaires on Smart Phone Addiction (SAS) scale, Bedtime Procrastination Scale (BPS), Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), and Depression, Anxiety Stress Scales 21 (DASS-21) across two different time points with six-months interval between them. The participants were selected using a cluster sampling technique from Quaid-e-Azam University, Islamabad, Pakistan. A cross-lagged model was utilized to assess the longitudinal association between these variables.
Results: Statistically significant reciprocal associations were found between PSU, bedtime procrastination, and mental health symptoms. PSU at Time 1 significantly predicted PSU at Time 2, bedtime procrastination at Time 2, sleep quality at Time 2, and mental health symptoms at Time 2. Bedtime procrastination at Time 1 predicted PSU at Time 2, sleep quality at Time 2, and mental health symptoms at Time 2. Sleep quality at Time 1 predicted bedtime procrastination at Time 2 and mental health symptoms at Time 2. Mental health symptoms at Time 1 predicted PSU at Time 2 and sleep quality at Time 2.
Conclusion: The research findings have significantly advanced understanding of the longitudinal connections between PSU, bedtime procrastination, sleep quality, and mental health indicators. This enhanced comprehension is instrumental for psychological practitioners in devising targeted interventions to mitigate such issues among the university student demographic.
Keywords: bedtime procrastination, problematic smartphone use, depression, anxiety, stress symptoms, sleep quality
Introduction
The notion of problematic smartphone use (PSU) elicits a certain level of controversy due to its relationship with the primary purpose of smartphones being the execution of internet-oriented applications.1 This relationship suggests that PSU shares several resemblances with internet-oriented addictions and might have reciprocal effects. Following earlier research, a significant association has been identified between internet addiction and PSU.2 In contrast, previous scholarly investigations have also demonstrated variations among these two groups regarding risk factors, including gender and personality traits.3 An instance of gender differentiation to internet addiction is observed, with men exhibiting a higher propensity towards this addictive behavior, in contrast to women who display a greater inclination towards PSU.4 Nevertheless, it should be acknowledged that smartphones offer many features encompassing not only internet connectivity and gaming capabilities but also a diverse range of other administrations and capacities, including communication capabilities, camera functionality, interactive media playback, artistic expression, and electronic book reading capabilities. Hence, it is evident that specific indicators of PSU may vary distinctively from those associated with internet addiction. Hence, existing scholarly studies predominantly consider PSU as a distinct theoretical construct, thereby creating various dedicated evaluation instruments that have gained broad acceptance and acknowledgment within the academic community.5–8 Nevertheless, it is essential to acknowledge that none of the currently available scales comprehensively account for the attributes of uncontrolled or excessive behavior, tolerance, and withdrawal when evaluating problematic smartphone use.8 Consequently, the present investigation employed the Smartphone Addiction Scale (SAS), a tool derived from the theoretical underpinnings of the Internet addiction scale (IAS), while accommodating the distinguishing features intrinsic to smartphones.6
Several empirical investigations have demonstrated a direct correlation between PSU and an individual’s overall health outcomes. PSU has been shown to not only result in physical symptoms9,10 but also to be a potential catalyst for mental health complications.11 Scholars have extensively investigated the symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress to both mental and physical well-being associated with PSU.11–14 For instance, an empirical investigation conducted on Australian adults revealed that high smartphone use had significantly higher depression, anxiety, and stress scores than those with lower use.12 Similarly, a comprehensive meta-analysis assessed the link between PSU and mental health issues across various populations. The results uniformly showed that such use is connected to substantial depression and anxiety symptoms, affirming evidence from different geographical areas.13 Another literature review incorporating a comprehensive meta-analysis conducted revealed a statistically significant and positive association between PSU and a diverse array of mental and somatic disorders.11 A cross-sectional survey conducted in multiple regions among 4200 individuals revealed a strong association between PSU and adverse mental health levels. The survey also provided insights into the types of activities people engage in with their smart phones.14 Furthermore, it is crucial to investigate the underlying mechanism that connects PSU and mental health symptoms and examine the immediate correlation between the two variables. Several scientific investigations have been conducted to scrutinize the mediators (resilience, neuroticism, reappraisal, borderline personality disorder) of the associations from self-regulation to personality characteristics.15–17 Nevertheless, most of these studies utilized cross-sectional approaches, thus neglecting to establish the causal directionality between the assorted variables. Consequently, comprehending the intricacies of the interplay between PSU, mental health symptoms, and additional potential mediating aspects becomes arduous. Exploring potential mechanisms in a relationship for which directionality and causality have not been adequately ascertained appears premature. Hence, prior to delving into the intricacies of this association’s mechanism, the expedient undertaking of longitudinal research is imperative to ascertain its directionality. Existing literature has centered on investigating the longitudinal associations between PSU and mental health symptoms.18–20 However, the findings of these investigations have yielded inconsistent and contradicted outcomes, suggesting a lack of consensus regarding the directionality of this relationship. Given the variation observed in the findings above, it is imperative to conduct longitudinal studies to delve deeper into the relationship.
Sleep quality may significantly influence the manifestation of PSU and mental health symptoms due to emerging evidence suggesting plausible biological and psychological interdependencies among these parameters. A study revealed that individuals in the adolescent demographic who suffer from sleep disorders exhibit a higher propensity to engage with smartphones for extended durations, as they utilize these devices as a coping mechanism to alleviate anxiety and concerns.21 This finding indirectly suggests that individuals who experience poor sleep quality may be at risk for developing PSU. Islam M. (2021) demonstrated that excessive smartphone use among South Korean adolescents is significantly associated with poor sleep quality, including delayed sleep onset, reduced sleep duration, and increased sleep disturbances.22 Another investigation among Serbian medical students showed that PSU and excessive screen time are linked to poorer sleep quality and more sleep disturbances.23 Prior research has examined the moderating influence of sleep quality in the relationship between PSU and mental health symptoms utilizing cross-sectional approaches across various populations from different regions.24–26 Nevertheless, incongruous findings have emerged through previous investigations, wherein mental health symptoms have served as a mediator between PSU and overall sleep quality.27 Furthermore, it is essential to note the limitations of cross-sectional designs, as these investigations have not yet provided conclusive evidence regarding the causal relationship between these factors. Previous longitudinal investigations have discovered reciprocal associations between PSU and sleep quality. More specifically, sleep quality has been identified as a predictor of PSU, predicted by sleep quality. Furthermore, these investigations have demonstrated that insufficient sleep and suboptimal sleep quality are predictive of subsequent mental health symptoms.28,29 However, it must be acknowledged that these pieces of evidence possess certain limitations, as their primary focus lies in examining the longitudinal association solely between the two variables, rather than delving into the intricate interplay among all three variables. The current body of literature regarding the correlation between PSU, sleep quality, and mental health symptoms remains inconclusive. Therefore, further longitudinal research is necessary to understand and comprehensively understand the relationship among these three variables.
The preceding literature has established that delaying bedtime, commonly called bedtime procrastination, mediates the constructive association between PSU and sleep quality among college and university students.30,31 Moreover, scholarly investigations have also revealed a correlation between trait procrastination, general procrastination, and PSU.32,33 The personality trait theory posits enduring characteristics or traits that can predict an individual’s behavior. Applying this notion to the trait of bedtime procrastination, it can be hypothesized that it may also play a role in the phenomenon of PSU. However, limited empirical investigation has been dedicated to exploring this potential association. Based on the procrastination-health paradigm, individuals with persistent tendencies to procrastinate will encounter stress resulting from failing to meet designated timeframes or completing tasks only at the eleventh hour.34 Consequently, they may engage in detrimental behaviors, such as utilizing their smartphones before bedtime, yielding instant gratification. Recently, there has been recognition of anxiety, stress, and unhealthy behaviors as significant determinants of reduced sleep duration and diminished sleep quality.35 Previous research has documented a connection between bedtime procrastination and sleep quality;30 however, it is worth noting that no prior studies have undertaken a longitudinal examination of the correlation between bedtime procrastination and sleep quality among college students with a comparatively more significant sample size in the Pakistani population.
In essence, it has been established that there are significant associations between PSU, bedtime procrastination, sleep quality, and mental health symptoms over time. Nevertheless, as mentioned earlier, prior research has primarily focused on conducting rudimentary correlation analyses involving a limited selection of two or three variables from the quartet. In addition, specific investigations solely encompassed systematic reviews without comprehensively systematically incorporating all four variables. Hence, the primary objective of this study is to investigate the longitudinal connections between PSU, bedtime procrastination, sleep quality, and mental health among university students in Pakistan.
Methods
Participants and Procedure
The researchers employed the stratified cluster sampling technique to gather data from university students. The sample was derived from 30 classes in a university in Islamabad, Pakistan. This university encompasses a broad range of disciplines and majors, including but not limited to science and engineering, humanities, social sciences, and medicine. A questionnaire survey was then conducted among the selected individuals. Through their cooperation, we contacted the university administration and acquired a comprehensive roster encompassing all courses. A sample of ten classes comprising first-year students, sophomores, and juniors was randomly chosen to administer the survey.
The inclusion criteria for the participants encompassed individuals who were 18 years of age or older, possessed a smartphone, lacked any familial background of mental disorders (anxiety or depression), and had not received any formal diagnosis of addictive disorders, affective, substance dependence, or, as indicated through self-reporting. In September 2021 (P1), 751 students were engaged in the initial assessment as part of the baseline study. In March 2022 (P2), the study achieved a participant count of 683 individuals, with an average age of 18.83 years and a standard deviation of 0.19. A follow-up survey was conducted, during which 68 participants from the original population at P1 were excluded due to their absence. The surveys were administered during classes within a designated timeframe of 30 minutes.
Ethics
Before conducting the survey, every student provided formal consent by signing an informed consent form. The study has received ethical approval from the Institutional Review Board of Quaid-e-Azam University (QAU/2021-May-0006712).
Questionnaire
Smartphone Addiction Scale- Short Version (SAS-SV)
The SAS is a 33-item scale with six distinct dimensions utilized to assess smartphone addiction;36 however, we have utilized a revised and short version of the scale, which has already been validated in the Pakistani population.37 The scale encompasses a range of options, extending from 1 to 6. Increased scores are indicative of an elevated susceptibility toward addiction to smartphones. The internal consistency test result for this scale in the present study was estimated to be 0.92 at P1 and 0.94 at P2.
Bedtime Procrastination Scale (BPS)
To assess the procrastination level among study participants, we employed a 9-item BPS developed by Kroese et al,38 a reliable and pre-validated instrument.39,40 The scale consists of 4 positively worded and 5 negatively worded items rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). Previous research has demonstrated a strong internal consistency of 0.79 for the BPS items.40,41 The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the present research study indicated a high level of internal consistency with a reliability coefficient of 0.94 at the baseline period (P1) and 0.96 at period P2 for the construct.
The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI)
The PSQI was used to measure the quality of sleep, as it provides a thorough evaluation of sleep by analyzing 19 characteristics that are divided into seven main aspects consisting of subjective sleep disturbance, sleep quality, use of sleeping medication, sleep latency, sleep duration, sleep efficiency, and daytime dysfunction.42 This investigation utilized a four-level Likert scale to allocate scores ranging from 0 to 3 for each dimension, yielding a cumulative score between 0–21. Participants with a higher score on the scale indicate poorer sleep quality. The measurement reliability of the scale was assessed using Cronbach’s alpha, which yielded coefficients of 0.73 and 0.74 at the two distinct time points (P1 and P2), respectively.
Depression Anxiety Stress Scales 21 (DASS-21)
We utilized a set of 21 self-report measures designed to assess the severity of core symptoms related to depression, anxiety, and stress, which has undergone prior validation and is widely recognized within the academic community.43 The response was recorded on a four-point Likert scale, with each participant’s total score on the Depression, Anxiety, and Stress scales calculated by multiplying the aggregate score of seven specific items by two. Each scale is subsequently divided into subscales of two to five items each. The reliability coefficients, as measured by Cranach’s alpha, indicate that the depression scale showed a score of 0.95 at P1 and 0.94 at P2, the anxiety scale showed a score of 0.86 at P1 and 0.89 at P2, whereas the stress scale showed a score of 0.94 at time P1 and 0.93 at P2.
Statistical Analyses
To obtain an objective evaluation of the data, we performed descriptive and Pearson correlation analysis by utilizing SPSS (v25), and the data is presented in the form of percentages for categorical variables. In contrast, numerical variables are represented using the mean value accompanied by the standard deviation (SD). The Mplus software (v7.4) was used to implement cross-lagged panel analysis. Initially, an assessment was made on the longitudinal measurement invariance of the four psychometric tools employed in this investigation, encompassing the evaluation of metric, scalar, and configural invariance.44 After controlling for sex and age variables, we established a cross-lagged model to examine the longitudinal bilateral correlations between PSU, bedtime procrastination, sleep quality, and mental health symptoms among the study participants. The adequacy of the model fit was assessed through a comprehensive examination of the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), comparative fit index (CFI), standard root means square residual (SRMR), and Tucker–Lewis’s index (TLI). Based on prior research, it has been determined that CFI and TLI exceeding a threshold of 0.90, along with RMSEA and SRMR below 0.08, signify an acceptable fit for the model under consideration.45 Due to the inherent sensitivity of X2 analysis to sample size, we chose to refrain from using it to measure model adequacy.46 In the interim, we estimated 95% confidence intervals (CI) employing a bias-corrected bootstrap technique that entailed 10,000 iterations. The estimated 95% CI reported herein does not encompass the value of zero, thereby illustrating the statistical significance of the observed effect at a p-value of less than 0.05. Furthermore, we employed ΔCFI and ΔRMSEA (Δ indicates the rate of change) values to assess the measurement invariance. The measurement invariance model was considered admissible when the difference in ΔCFI was less than or equal to 0.01, and the difference in ΔRMSEA was less than or equal to 0.015.47
Results
To examine the long-term measurement invariance of all the psychometric measures, we initially constructed configural invariance models. The findings demonstrated that the configural invariance models fit all four measures satisfactorily (Table 1). Following this, we ensured that the factor loadings remained consistent across time and implemented models for metric invariance. All of the models fit and exhibited satisfactory accuracy. The findings of the metric invariance models revealed that both ΔCFI and ΔRMSEA exhibited values ≤ 0.01, which suggests the preservation of factor loadings on each measure throughout the temporal dimension. According to the metric invariance model, we imposed additional constraints on threshold equality to evaluate scalar invariance. The changes in ΔCFI and ΔRMSEA remained within the permissible range. The findings of this study suggest that the four measures demonstrated consistency in their measurements across two separate periods.
![]() |
Table 1 Measurement Invariance Over Time |
The statistical summary of the study variables is presented in Table 2, which includes the means, standard deviations, and Pearson’s correlations. The findings indicated the existence of statistically significant associations between the four psychometric measures, ie, PSU, bedtime procrastination, quality of sleep, and depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms, assessed at two distinct periods (P1 and P2).
![]() |
Table 2 Descriptive and Correlational Analyses of the Selected Psychometric Measures in the Study |
The results of the adopted cross-lagged model in the current investigation are presented in Figure 1 and Table 3. The findings highlight a positive correlation between PSU and bedtime procrastination at P1 and reciprocal associations between PSU and mental health symptoms at time point P2. In contrast, sleep quality demonstrated significant predictive power in only one direction. The present study found that baseline bedtime procrastination at P1 was positively associated with sleep quality at P2. Conversely, sleep quality at P1 was positively associated with bedtime procrastination at P2. Additionally, bedtime procrastination was found to significantly and positively predict depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms in one direction. Furthermore, it was found that the quality of sleep observed at P1 showed a positive association with depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms noted at P2, and similarly, depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms at P1 were also predictive of sleep quality at P2.
![]() |
Table 3 Cross-Lagged Analysis Results |
![]() |
Figure 1 A longitudinal cross-lagged path model among study variables across two-time frames ie P1 & P2. **Significant at 0.01. ***Significant at 0.001. |
Discussion
In the present investigation, a two-wave longitudinal design was employed to examine the reciprocal relationships between PSU, bedtime procrastination, sleep quality, and mental health symptoms. Additionally, a cross-lagged model was constructed to analyze these connections. These findings contribute to comprehending the longitudinal associations and causal relationships among the aforementioned psychometric measures. The findings indicated several longitudinal associations in both directions. Firstly, there is a significant relationship between PSU and both bedtime procrastination and mental health symptoms. Secondly, sleep quality is associated with bedtime procrastination and mental health symptoms. This study represents the initial examination in which bedtime procrastination has been linked with PSU and sleep quality. Additionally, specific unidirectional associations were observed in the study outcomes. Specifically, PSU demonstrated a predictive relationship with sleep quality, while bedtime procrastination exhibited a predictive relationship with mental health symptoms.
The findings of this study indicate a bidirectional relationship between PSU, sleep quality, and mental health symptoms, which aligns with several prior research findings.48–51 Nevertheless, a recent investigation conducted in Pakistan spanning six months revealed that the presence of anxiety and stress symptoms at the initial assessment was indicative of subsequent psychological well-being issues, in contrast to the inverse relationship.48 One potential explanation arises from the divergent lengths of follow-up periods, which may contribute to disparate manifestations of effects among PSU and mental health symptoms. Two preceding longitudinal investigations, lasting more than one year each, revealed a reciprocal connection between PSU and mental health symptoms.52,53 Nonetheless, a short-term longitudinal investigation conducted among American students over three months revealed a unidirectional correlation, wherein baseline PSU was found to be predictive of subsequent depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms. This hypothesis suggests that discerning the reciprocal connection between the two variables may necessitate a lengthier period. Longitudinal investigations over a year have uncovered a unidirectional relationship.54,55 Hence, it is imperative to undertake additional longitudinal research endeavors, incorporating extended follow-up durations, to elucidate the enduring correlation between PSU and mental health symptoms. Our findings strongly indicate the significance of developing efficacious interventions to mitigate PSU and mental health symptoms, underscoring the pressing need for immediate attention. Furthermore, there is substantial evidence that supports the association between sleep disorders and mental health symptoms. In the study conducted by Breslau et al, it was observed that sleep disruption served as a significant predictor for symptoms associated with mental health among individuals in the young adult population.56
Interestingly, many research investigations have established significant biological associations between these variables.57,58 Furthermore, several empirical investigations have uncovered that sleep quality is a mediating factor in the relationship between PSU and physical health.59–61 These findings support the assertion that a significant correlation exists between sleep quality and mental health symptoms. Our investigation additionally ascertained that PSU demonstrated the capability to predict sleep quality solely in unidirectional, thus corroborating the findings of prior longitudinal investigations.62 Previous research has established a connection between PSU and various sleep-related issues. For instance, a three-year longitudinal survey on Korean adults unveiled a link between PSU and sleep quality.62 Moreover, a cross-sectional study demonstrated that utilizing smartphones for over 5 hours regularly correlated with inadequate sleeping duration and increased prevalence of insomnia among high school students.63 The present research findings do not support the hypothesis that sleep quality at P1 can predict PSU at P2. This finding contrasts prior evidence that reported a bidirectional association.55 Existing evidence has demonstrated a significant association between suboptimal nocturnal sleep quality and the manifestation of excessive tiredness and somnolence during the daytime.64–66 The findings suggest that college students exhibiting suboptimal sleep quality may potentially encounter feelings of daytime fatigue and somnolence, manifesting in limited availability of temporal and cognitive resources for engaging with mobile devices. This subsequently contributes to reducing both the prevalence and duration of smartphone usage, thereby attenuating the likelihood of PSU.
Our findings revealed a bidirectional association between bedtime procrastination and PSU and sleep quality. This finding further substantiates the perspective put forth by Sirois et al, positing that bedtime procrastination heightens the health hazards individuals face.67 Despite the absence of any existing reports in academic literature, a cross-sectional study conducted a preliminary examination of the causal relationships involved using a mediation model. The findings suggest that PSU indirectly influences sleep quality by affecting bedtime procrastination.68 Steel’s Temporary Motivation Theory (TMT) posits that the tendency for procrastination in individuals is contingent upon their perception of the utility of the task. Furthermore, this multifaceted variable is intricately linked to value, expectation, and delay.69 Individuals with PSU demonstrated an extended duration for acquiring health advantages derived from sleep compared to the timeframe during which immediate psychological satisfaction and pleasure were gained through smartphone usage. Consequently, the former option was significantly less appealing to individuals than the latter alternative. This can be attributed to the fact that individuals who are addicted to smartphones tend to postpone sleep-related activities in favor of activities that are perceived to offer comparatively greater utility, ultimately leading to the phenomenon known as bedtime procrastination. Conversely, adopting bedtime procrastination as a habitual practice signifies that individuals are afforded a greater allocation of time and avail themselves of additional prospects for participating in alternative pursuits. For instance, individuals who procrastinate at bedtime may predominantly opt for smartphone usage, as it offers high accessibility and a range of powerful features. Smartphones increase the likelihood of experiencing PSU 71 by obtaining instantaneous gratification and contentment.
Furthermore, a comprehensive longitudinal investigation has further demonstrated that academic procrastination is a precursor to PSU among middle school students.70 The present findings suggest that a reciprocal causal relationship may exist between PSU and bedtime procrastination, leading to a cyclic pattern of detriment—the statement above held validity about the correlation between bedtime procrastination and sleep quality. Bedtime procrastination has the potential to induce sleep disturbances and decrease subjective sleep duration. Simultaneously, individuals who procrastinate are compelled to voluntarily endure the stress of retiring to bed at the final moment and subsequently achieving prompt sleep onset, decreasing sleep quality. Prior research has also identified that individuals with low self-control can deplete their energy and capacity for self-regulation, leading to a heightened risk of inadequate self-regulation in the workplace. Consequently, this deficiency may contribute to the propensity for work procrastination.71 The predictive relationship between sleep quality at P1 and bedtime procrastination at P2 can potentially be accounted for by the association between procrastination and the inadequacy of self-regulatory efforts elucidated by the self-regulation perspective on procrastination.72 Hence, individuals exhibiting suboptimal sleep quality may encounter difficulties in maintaining self-regulation. This can manifest in their susceptibility to being enticed by alternative stimuli or engagements before their sleeping routine, thereby contributing to bedtime procrastination.71
Moreover, our findings indicate that the presence of bedtime procrastination at P1 is a significant predictor of the subsequent manifestation of mental health symptoms at P2. Previous evidence has yielded substantial evidence indicating a noteworthy bidirectional association between the engagement of adolescents in self-harming behaviors, known as bedtime procrastination, and the subsequent development of mental health symptoms in the subsequent year.73 Nevertheless, the correlation between mental health symptoms and the subsequent development of bedtime procrastination did not yield statistically significant results. This discrepancy is potentially attributed to the dissimilarities observed in using bedtime procrastination evaluation instruments across the two investigations. Prior research primarily relied on self-report measures to assess bedtime procrastination; however, in this current study, a specialized scale was utilized to measure this phenomenon. Hence, additional investigation is warranted to investigate the longitudinal correlation between bedtime procrastination and mental health symptoms. Procrastination has been identified as a phenomenon described by the prioritization of one’s immediate self-interest over future-oriented considerations and the prioritization of short-term emotional regulation over long-haul objectives and benefits, as observed along the unidirectional trajectory from bedtime procrastination to mental health symptoms.74
Individuals experiencing bedtime procrastination may realize that failing to adhere to proper sleeping schedules can negatively impact their health. As a result, they may impose self-inflicted pressure on themselves, subsequently leading to the manifestation of symptoms associated with mental health. Prior research indicates a potential link between bedtime procrastination and the onset of detrimental psychological outcomes.31,40 These adverse psychological effects may ultimately manifest in the emergence of mental health symptoms. Furthermore, it should be noted that bedtime procrastination has the potential to exert an indirect influence on the manifestation of mental health symptoms via its impact on the individual’s sleep quality. Specifically, bedtime procrastination is liable to diminish the overall sleep quality experienced by the individual, thereby exacerbating the likelihood of mental health emergence. This suggests that there may be a prospective mediating role of sleep quality in the relationship between behavioral and psychological symptoms and daily activity functioning. However, additional rigorous multi-level longitudinal investigations are needed to confirm this hypothesis.
Limitations and Future Directions
Despite the valuable insights provided by this study on the longitudinal associations between PSU, bedtime procrastination, sleep quality, and mental health symptoms among university students, it is imperative to acknowledge its limitations and delineate future research directions. Firstly, the sample was derived from a single institution, which may limit the generalizability of the findings to other educational contexts or demographic groups. Future studies could benefit from a more diverse participant pool, including varied age groups, occupations, and cultural backgrounds, to ascertain the universality of these relationships. Secondly, self-report measures were employed in this research, which could introduce response biases such as social desirability or recall inaccuracies. Objective measures of smartphone usage patterns and sleep quality (eg, actigraphy) could complement self-report data in future investigations, offering a more holistic understanding of these behaviors.
Moreover, the study’s design does not allow for causal inferences; while associations are identified, the directionality and underlying mechanisms remain unclear. Experimental or longitudinal studies with multiple waves of data collection could elucidate the causal pathways between these variables. Lastly, this study focused on adverse outcomes associated with problematic smartphone use and bedtime procrastination but did not explore potential moderating or mediating factors that could mitigate these effects. Future research could examine protective factors such as resilience, mindfulness, or social support, which might buffer against the adverse consequences of these behaviors on mental health.
Conclusion
This research contributes significantly to the body of knowledge regarding the longitudinal and reciprocal associations between PSU, bedtime procrastination, sleep quality, and mental health symptoms among university students. Our study provides a more refined understanding of the complex interplay between these factors by employing a robust methodology that assesses these variables across two-time points. The findings underscore the importance of PSU as a salient factor influencing both sleep quality and mental health outcomes. They highlight that the relationship between PSU and these variables is not unidirectional but demonstrates persistent reciprocal influences over time. This insight is crucial for mental health professionals, particularly those working within educational settings, as it enables them to develop more targeted and timely interventions.
Moreover, the study emphasizes the need to address bedtime procrastination as a significant concern linked to PSU and adverse mental health symptoms. The recognition of this behavior’s impact on sleep quality and mental well-being adds a new dimension to the discussion on health outcomes related to technology use. Furthermore, while our study acknowledges the ongoing debate regarding classifying PSU as a disorder, it provides empirical evidence supporting the notion that excessive smartphone use correlates with psychological and physical impairments. This correlation, however, may not be directly proportional, suggesting a more nuanced relationship that warrants further exploration. In addition, our research supplements the dimensional methodology employed in investigating PSU. By considering multiple dimensions of mental health—including depression, anxiety, and stress—our study provides a broader perspective on the mental health implications of PSU and associated behaviors. Finally, the insights gained from this research will aid educational professionals specializing in mental health within schools in formulating precise interventions aimed at mitigating problematic patterns of PSU, sleep difficulties, and symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress within the university student population. Such interventions could include but are not limited to, digital wellness workshops, cognitive-behavioral therapy for technology-related issues, and promoting healthy sleep habits.
In conclusion, our study not only enhances the existing knowledge in the field but also paves the way for future research to explore potential moderating or mediating factors that could further elucidate the complexities of the relationships identified. We hope this line of inquiry will continue to evolve, shedding light on the intricate ways technology use affects the academic, psychological, and physical well-being of university students.
Data Sharing Statement
The original data used to support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. (Shazia Rehman: [email protected]).
Ethical Approval
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Research Ethics Board of the Quaid-e-Azam University and with the 1964 helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards (QAU/2021-May-0006712).
Informed Consent
Informed consent was obtained from all participants in the study.
Funding
This study received APC funding from the University of North Texas, Denton, TX, USA.
Disclosure
Dr Shyamkumar Sriram is affiliated with Department of Rehabilitation and Health Services, College of Health and Public Service, University of North Texas, Denton, 76201, Texas, USA since August 16, 2024. The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest in this work.
References
1. Mok J-Y, Choi S-W, Kim D-J, et al. Latent class analysis on internet and smartphone addiction in college students. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2014;10:817. doi:10.2147/NDT.S59293
2. Ben-Yehuda L, Greenberg L, Weinstein A. Internet addiction by using the smartphone-relationships between internet addiction, frequency of smartphone use and the state of mind of male and female students. J Reward Defic Syndr Addict Sci. 2016;2(1):22–27. doi:10.17756/jrdsas.2016-024
3. Jin Jeong Y, Suh B, Gweon G. Is smartphone addiction different from Internet addiction? Comparison of addiction-risk factors among adolescents. Behav Inf Technol. 2020;39(5):578–593. doi:10.1080/0144929X.2019.1604805
4. Lee S-Y, Lee D, Nam CR, et al. Distinct patterns of Internet and smartphone-related problems among adolescents by gender: latent class analysis. J Behav Addict. 2018;7(2):454–465. doi:10.1556/2006.7.2018.28
5. Lin Y-H, Chang L-R, Lee Y-H, Tseng H-W, Kuo TBJ, Chen S-H. Development and validation of the Smartphone Addiction Inventory (SPAI). PLoS One. 2014;9(6):e98312. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0098312
6. Kwon M, Lee J-Y, Won W-Y, et al. Development and validation of a smartphone addiction scale (SAS). PLoS One. 2013;8(2):e56936. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0056936
7. Kim D, Lee Y, Lee J, Nam JK, Chung Y. Development of Korean smartphone addiction proneness scale for youth. PLoS One. 2014;9(5):e97920. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0097920
8. Merlo LJ, Stone AM, Bibbey A. Measuring problematic mobile phone use: development and preliminary psychometric properties of the PUMP scale. J Addict. 2013;2013:1–7. doi:10.1155/2013/912807
9. Mustafaoglu R, Yasaci Z, Zirek E, Griffiths MD, Ozdincler AR. The relationship between smartphone addiction and musculoskeletal pain prevalence among young population: a cross-sectional study. Korean J Pain. 2021;34(1):72–81. doi:10.3344/kjp.2021.34.1.72
10. Suliman A, Jawaher T, Mohamed A, et al. The association between mobile phone use and severe traffic injuries: a case-control study from Saudi Arabia. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(15):1.
11. Yang J, Fu X, Liao X, Li Y. Association of problematic smartphone use with poor sleep quality, depression, and anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2020;284:112686. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2019.112686
12. Khan A, McLeod G, Hidajat T, Edwards EJ. Excessive smartphone use is associated with depression, anxiety, stress, and sleep quality of Australian adults. J Med Syst. 2023;47(1):109. doi:10.1007/s10916-023-02005-3
13. Augner C, Vlasak T, Aichhorn W, Barth A. The association between problematic smartphone use and symptoms of anxiety and depression—a meta-analysis. J Public Health. 2023;45(1):193–201. doi:10.1093/pubmed/fdab350
14. Kliestik T, Scott J, Musa H, Suler P. Addictive smartphone behavior, anxiety symptom severity, and depressive stress. Anal Metaphys. 2020;19:45–51.
15. Che Y, Fang D, Cai L, et al. Mediation of resilience in the association between personality traits and suicidal ideation among Chinese adolescents. Front Psychol. 2022;2022:13.
16. Kobylińska D, Zajenkowski M, Lewczuk K, Jankowski KS, Marchlewska M. The mediational role of emotion regulation in the relationship between personality and subjective well-being. Curr Psychol. 2022;41(6):4098–4111. doi:10.1007/s12144-020-00861-7
17. Akyunus M, Gençöz T, Karakose S. The mediator role of negative cognitive emotion regulation strategies between interpersonal problems and borderline personality beliefs. J Ration Cogn Ther. 2021;39(3):322–334. doi:10.1007/s10942-020-00373-0
18. Yang X, Hu H, Zhao C, Xu H, Tu X, Zhang G. A longitudinal study of changes in smart phone addiction and depressive symptoms and potential risk factors among Chinese college students. BMC Psychiatry. 2021;21(1):252. doi:10.1186/s12888-021-03265-4
19. Zhao C, He J, Du M, et al. A cross-lagged study of the associations among problematic smartphone use, depressive symptoms, and suicidal ideation in Chinese university students. Curr Psychol. 2024;43(18):16796–16807. doi:10.1007/s12144-023-04560-x
20. Cui G, Yin Y, Li S, et al. Longitudinal relationships among problematic mobile phone use, bedtime procrastination, sleep quality and depressive symptoms in Chinese college students: a cross-lagged panel analysis. BMC Psychiatry. 2021;21(1):449. doi:10.1186/s12888-021-03451-4
21. Elhai JD, Levine JC, Hall BJ. The relationship between anxiety symptom severity and problematic smartphone use: a review of the literature and conceptual frameworks. J Anxiety Disord. 2019;62:45–52. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2018.11.005
22. Islam M. Link between excessive smartphone use and sleeping disorders and depression among South Korean University students. Healthcare. 2021;9:1213. doi:10.3390/healthcare9091213
23. Nikolic A, Bukurov B, Kocic I, et al. Smartphone addiction, sleep quality, depression, anxiety, and stress among medical students. Front Public Health. 2023;11:1252371. doi:10.3389/fpubh.2023.1252371
24. Zou L, Wu X, Tao S, et al. Mediating effect of sleep quality on the relationship between problematic mobile phone use and depressive symptoms in college students. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10:822. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00822
25. Aabrooi S, Shafiqueii S, Javediii A, Fatimaiv A, Khanv O, RiazVi S. Frequency of cervicogenic headache in students due to usage of smart devices-cross sectional survey. Pakistan J Rehabil. 2022;11(2):36–41. doi:10.36283/pjr.zu.11.2/007
26. Mandhwani S, Zia S, Shaikh ES, Duarte D, Tanveer E. Association between cell phone usage and musculoskeletal disorders in school going children. J Musculoskelet Res. 2022;25(01):2250003. doi:10.1142/S0218957722500038
27. Demirci K, Akgönül M, Akpinar A. Relationship of smartphone use severity with sleep quality, depression, and anxiety in university students. J Behav Addict. 2015;4(2):85–92. doi:10.1556/2006.4.2015.010
28. Wang W, Du X, Guo Y, et al. The associations between sleep situations and mental health among Chinese adolescents: a longitudinal study. Sleep Med. 2021;82:71–77. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2021.03.009
29. Peltz JS, Rogge RD, Bodenlos JS, Kingery JN, Pigeon WR. Changes in psychological inflexibility as a potential mediator of longitudinal links between college students’ sleep problems and depressive symptoms. J Context Behav Sci. 2020;15:110–122. doi:10.1016/j.jcbs.2019.12.003
30. Ghafoor RZ, Nawaz S, Zahra T, Hakeem TA. Effect of smartphone addiction on academic performance; Mediation of self-regulation and bedtime procrastination. Pakistan J Med Heal Sci. 2022;16(09):618. doi:10.53350/pjmhs22169618
31. Rehman S, Rehman E, Liu B. Potential correlation between self-compassion and bedtime procrastination: the mediating role of emotion regulation. Psychol Res Behav Manag. 2023;16(1):4709–4723. doi:10.2147/PRBM.S431922
32. Rozgonjuk D, Kattago M, Täht K. Social media use in lectures mediates the relationship between procrastination and problematic smartphone use. Comput Human Behav. 2018;89:191–198. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.003
33. Li L, Gao H, Xu Y. The mediating and buffering effect of academic self-efficacy on the relationship between smartphone addiction and academic procrastination. Comput Educ. 2020;159:104001. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2020.104001
34. Sirois FM, Melia-Gordon ML, Pychyl TA. “I’ll look after my health, later”: an investigation of procrastination and health. Pers Individ Dif. 2003;35(5):1167–1184. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00326-4
35. Li X, Buxton OM, Kim Y, Haneuse S, Kawachi I. Do procrastinators get worse sleep? Cross-sectional study of US adolescents and young adults. SSM-Population Heal. 2020;10:100518. doi:10.1016/j.ssmph.2019.100518
36. Kwon M, Kim D-J, Cho H, Yang S. The smartphone addiction scale: development and validation of a short version for adolescents. PLoS One. 2013;8(12):e83558. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0083558
37. Khalily MT, Loona MI, Bhatti MM, Ahmad I, Saleem T. Smartphone addiction and its associated factors among students in twin cities of Pakistan. JPMA. 2020;70(8):1357–1362.
38. Kroese FM, De Ridder DTD, Evers C, Adriaanse MA. Bedtime procrastination: introducing a new area of procrastination. Front Psychol. 2014;5:89333. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00611
39. Kroese FM, Nauts S, Kamphorst BA, Anderson JH, de Ridder DTD. Bedtime procrastination: a behavioral perspective on sleep insufficiency. Procrast Health Well-Being Elsevier. 2016;2016:93–119.
40. Kroese FM, Evers C, Adriaanse MA, de Ridder DTD. Bedtime procrastination: a self-regulation perspective on sleep insufficiency in the general population. J Health Psychol. 2016;21(5):853–862. doi:10.1177/1359105314540014
41. Hill VM, Rebar AL, Ferguson SA, Shriane AE, Vincent GE. Go to bed! A systematic review and meta-analysis of bedtime procrastination correlates and sleep outcomes. Sleep Med Rev. 2022;66:101697. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2022.101697
42. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 1989;28(2):193–213. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4
43. Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH. The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav Res Ther. 1995;33(3):335–343. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U
44. Meredith W. Measurement invariance, factor analysis and factorial invariance. Psychometrika. 1993;58:525–543. doi:10.1007/BF02294825
45. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model a Multidiscip J. 1999;6(1):1–55. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118
46. Schermelleh-Engel K, Moosbrugger H, Müller H. Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods Psychol Res. 2003;8(2):23–74.
47. Chen FF. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct Equ Model a Multidiscip J. 2007;14(3):464–504. doi:10.1080/10705510701301834
48. Urieta P, Sorrel MA, Aluja A, Balada F, Lacomba E, García LF. Exploring the relationship between personality, decision-making styles, and problematic smartphone use. Curr Psychol. 2022;2022:1–18.
49. Zhong Y, Ma H, Liang Y-F, Liao C-J, Zhang -C-C, Jiang W-J. Prevalence of smartphone addiction among Asian medical students: a meta-analysis of multinational observational studies. Int J Soc Psychiatry. 2022;68(6):1171–1183. doi:10.1177/00207640221089535
50. Green M, Kovacova M, Valaskova K. Smartphone addiction risk, depression psychopathology, and social anxiety. Anal Metaphys. 2020;19:52–58.
51. Lăzăroiu G, Kovacova M, Siekelova A, Vrbka J. Addictive behavior of problematic smartphone users: the relationship between depression, anxiety, and stress. Rev Contemp Philos. 2020;19:50–56.
52. Park S-Y, Yang S, Shin C-S, Jang H, Park S-Y. Long-term symptoms of mobile phone use on mobile phone addiction and depression among Korean adolescents. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(19):3584. doi:10.3390/ijerph16193584
53. Jun S. The reciprocal longitudinal relationships between mobile phone addiction and depressive symptoms among Korean adolescents. Comput Human Behav. 2016;58:179–186. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.12.061
54. Coyne SM, Stockdale L, Summers K. Problematic cell phone use, depression, anxiety, and self-regulation: evidence from a three year longitudinal study from adolescence to emerging adulthood. Comput Human Behav. 2019;96:78–84. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2019.02.014
55. Kang Y, Liu S, Yang L, et al. Testing the bidirectional associations of mobile phone addiction behaviors with mental distress, sleep disturbances, and sleep patterns: a one-year prospective study among Chinese college students. Front Psychiatry. 2020;11:634. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00634
56. Breslau N, Roth T, Rosenthal L, Andreski P. Sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders: a longitudinal epidemiological study of young adults. Biol Psychiatry. 1996;39(6):411–418. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(95)00188-3
57. Baglioni C, Nanovska S, Regen W, et al. Sleep and mental disorders: a meta-analysis of polysomnographic research. Psychol Bull. 2016;142(9):969. doi:10.1037/bul0000053
58. Rehman S, Addas A, Rahman MA, Shahiman MA, Li Z. Sequential mediation analysis of physical activity, healthy diet, BMI, and academic burnout in the Pakistani educational landscape. Sci Rep. 2024;14(1):7737. doi:10.1038/s41598-024-58126-3
59. Stanković M, Nešić M, Čičević S, Shi Z. Association of smartphone use with depression, anxiety, stress, sleep quality, and internet addiction. Empirical evidence from a smartphone application. Pers Individ Dif. 2021;168:110342. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2020.110342
60. Rafeeq S, Naman MS, Ijaz MT, Touseef M, Rai IA, Chaudhry MA. Association of excessive smartphone use with sleep in students of a private medical college in Lahore, Pakistan. Rawal Med J. 2021;46(4):947–950.
61. Ejaz W, Altay S, Naeem G. Smartphone use and well-being in Pakistan: comparing the effect of self-reported and actual smartphone use. Digit Heal. 2023;9:20552076231186076.
62. Lee JE, Jang S-I, Ju YJ, Kim W, Lee HJ, Park E-C. Relationship between Mobile phone addiction and the incidence of poor and short sleep among Korean adolescents: a longitudinal study of the Korean Children & Youth Panel Survey. J Korean Med Sci. 2017;32(7):1166–1172. doi:10.3346/jkms.2017.32.7.1166
63. Tamura H, Nishida T, Tsuji A, Sakakibara H. Association between excessive use of mobile phone and insomnia and depression among Japanese adolescents. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017;14(7):701. doi:10.3390/ijerph14070701
64. Zaman M, Babar MS, Babar M, et al. Prevalence of gaming addiction and its impact on sleep quality: a cross-sectional study from Pakistan. Ann Med Surg. 2022;78:103641. doi:10.1016/j.amsu.2022.103641
65. Surani AA, Zahid S, Surani A, Ali S, Mubeen M, Khan RH. Sleep quality among medical students of Karachi, Pakistan. J Pak Med Assoc. 2015;65(4):380–382.
66. Bokhari NM, Zafar M. Daytime sleepiness and sleep quality among undergraduate Medical students in Sialkot, Pakistan. Dr Sulaiman Al Habib Med J. 2020;2(2):51–55. doi:10.2991/dsahmj.k.200514.002
67. Sirois FM. Procrastination and stress: a conceptual review of why context matters. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2023;20(6):5031. doi:10.3390/ijerph20065031
68. Mehmood H, Siddiqui DA. The mediatory role of active, and passive procrastination in the impact of personality traits, and other factors on students’ academic performance and well-being in higher education institutions of Pakistan. Available SSRN. 2023;2023:1.
69. Steel P. The nature of procrastination: a meta-analytic and theoretical review of quintessential self-regulatory failure. Psychol Bull. 2007;133(1):65. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.65
70. Iftikhar A, Liaquat AW, Shahid H. Mediating effect of academic amotivation between smartphone addiction and academic procrastination among university students. Online Media Soc. 2022;3:202–212.
71. Van Eerde W, Venus M. A daily diary study on sleep quality and procrastination at work: the moderating role of trait self-control. Front Psychol. 2018;9:2029. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02029
72. Tice DM, Baumeister RF. Longitudinal study of procrastination, performance, stress, and health: the costs and benefits of dawdling. Psychol Sci. 1997;8(6):454–458. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00460.x
73. Tochigi M, Usami S, Matamura M, et al. Annual longitudinal survey at up to five time points reveals reciprocal effects of bedtime delay and depression/anxiety in adolescents. Sleep Med. 2016;17:81–86. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2015.08.024
74. Sirois F, Pychyl T. Procrastination and the priority of short‐term mood regulation: consequences for future self. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2013;7(2):115–127. doi:10.1111/spc3.12011
© 2024 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The
full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php
and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution
- Non Commercial (unported, 3.0) License.
By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted
without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly
attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.