Back to Journals » International Journal of Nephrology and Renovascular Disease » Volume 18
Causal Relationship Between Intelligence, Noncognitive Education, Cognition and Urinary Tract or Kidney Infection: A Mendelian Randomization Study
Authors Fu S , Li Q, Cheng L, Wan S, Wang Q, Min Y, Xie Y, Liu H, Hu T, Liu H , Chen W, Zhang Y, Xiong F
Received 12 December 2024
Accepted for publication 25 February 2025
Published 7 March 2025 Volume 2025:18 Pages 71—85
DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/IJNRD.S511736
Checked for plagiarism Yes
Review by Single anonymous peer review
Peer reviewer comments 2
Editor who approved publication: Professor Pravin Singhal
Shuai Fu,* Qiang Li,* Li Cheng, Sheng Wan, Quan Wang, Yonglong Min, Yanghao Xie, Huizhen Liu, Taotao Hu, Hong Liu, Weidong Chen, Yanmin Zhang, Fei Xiong
Department of Nephrology, Wuhan No. 1 Hospital, Wuhan, Hubei Province, People’s Republic of China
*These authors contributed equally to this work
Correspondence: Fei Xiong; Yanmin Zhang, Department of Nephrology, Wuhan No. 1 Hospital, No. 215 Zhongshan Avenue, Wuhan, Hubei Province, 430022, People’s Republic of China, Email [email protected]; [email protected]
Background: The occurrence of urinary tract or kidney infection is correlated with intelligence, noncognitive education and cognition, but the causal relationship between them remains uncertain, and which risk factors mediate this causal relationship remains unknown.
Methods: The intelligence (n=269,867), noncognitive education (n=510,795) and cognition data (n=257,700) were obtained from genome-wide association studies (GWAS) conducted in individuals of European ethnicities. The genetic associations between these factors and urinary tract or kidney infection (UK Biobank, n=397,867) were analyzed using linkage disequilibrium score regression. We employed a two-sample univariate and multivariate Mendelian randomization to evaluate the causal relationship, and utilized a two-step Mendelian randomization to examine the involvement of 28 potential mediators and their respective mediating proportions.
Results: The genetic correlation coefficients of intelligence, noncognitive education, cognition, and urinary tract or kidney infection were − 0.338, − 0.218, and − 0.330. The Mendelian randomization using the inverse variance weighted method revealed each 1-SD increase in intelligence, the risk of infection decreased by 15.9%, while after adjusting for noncognitive education, the risk decreased by 20%. For each 1-SD increase in noncognitive education, the risk of infection decreased by 8%, which further reduced to 7.1% after adjusting for intelligence and to 6.7% after adjusting for cognition. For each 1-SD increase in cognition, the risk of infection decreased by 10.8%, increasing to 11.9% after adjusting for noncognitive education. The effects of intelligence and cognition are interdependent. 2 out of 28 potential mediating factors exhibited significant mediation effects in the causal relationship between noncognitive education and urinary tract or kidney infection, with body mass index accounting for 12.1% of the mediation effect and smoking initiation accounting for 14.7%.
Conclusion: Enhancing intelligence, noncognitive education, and cognition can mitigate the susceptibility to urinary tract or kidney infection. Noncognitive education exhibited independent effect, while body mass index and smoking initiation assuming a mediating role.
Keywords: cognition, intelligence, kidney infection, mediation analyses, Mendelian randomization, noncognitive education, urinary tract infection
Introduction
Urinary tract infections (UTIs) pose a significant threat to global public health, affecting over 150 million individuals annually across various demographics.1 These infections can manifest in both genders and all age groups, presenting treatment challenges such as recurrent episodes, pathogen diversity, and the emergence of antibiotic resistance.2 Severe UTIs may ultimately result in kidney failure.3 Intelligence, education, and cognition exert a pervasive influence on all facets of life and serve as significant prognosticators for socioeconomic attainment, health outcomes, and longevity.4,5 Prospective cohort study has demonstrated a significant association between childhood intelligence quotient (IQ) and the levels of infectious inflammatory markers, including interleukin-6 and C-reactive protein.6 Cross-sectional study has demonstrated that pregnant women with lower educational attainment are at a higher risk of developing UTIs.7 Case-control study has demonstrated that elderly patients with cognitive dysfunction exhibit more pronounced pro-inflammatory responses during acute bacterial infections including UTIs, which is evidenced by decreased expression of miR-145 in circulating exosomes and increased expression of CR1 in circulating CD14 monocytes.8 However, the causal relationship between these factors and UTIs remains uncertain. Recent studies have established correlations between genetic variation and intelligence, education, and cognition levels.9,10 Analyzing genetic factors and lifestyle data indicated that intelligence, education, and cognition collectively influence various life outcomes, while also exhibiting relatively independent effects.11 The objective of this study is to investigate the causal effects of intelligence, noncognitive education, and cognition on urinary tract or kidney infection from genetic variation perspective in order to enhance our understanding of the pathogenesis of UTIs and provide guidance for prevention and management measures.
Mendelian randomization (MR) utilizes the random assignment of individual genotypes from ancestral genotypes to explore causal inference in etiological epidemiology.12 The utilization of genetic variation as instrumental variables facilitates the examination of the causal relationship between exposure and outcome, while minimizing the bias of confounding factors. Comparable to randomized assignment trials, this approach is extensively employed and offers cost-effective benefits. We employed univariate Mendelian randomization (UVMR) to examine the total causal association between each exposure and outcome, conducted multivariate Mendelian randomization (MVMR) to assess the independent effects of each exposure while accounting for mutual correction in the same instrumental variable model,13 and utilized 2-step UVMR to investigate the mediating factors in the causal relationship.14 The subject of MR in relation to our topic has not been previously explored, and as far as our knowledge goes, we are the pioneers in utilizing this methodology to investigate the causality within this subject.
Methods
Study Design
The MR framework is based on three fundamental assumptions: (1) The association hypothesis posits a robust correlation between genetic variables and at least one exposure; (2) The independence hypothesis assumes that genetic variables remain unaffected by all confounding factors associated with the exposure and outcome, such as environmental influences; (3) The pleiotropy hypothesis suggests that genetic variables can solely influence outcomes through one or more exposures, without any horizontal pleiotropic effects. This study encompasses 3 stages of analysis (study design shown in Figure 1). In stage 1, we employed UVMR to examine the total causal effects of intelligence, noncognitive education, and cognition on urinary tract or kidney infection. In stage 2, we utilized MVMR to investigate the independent causal effects of intelligence, noncognitive education, and cognition in the same model that accounted for mutual adjustment for urinary tract or kidney infection. In stage 3, we adopted a two-step UVMR to analyze the mediating factors and quantify the proportion of mediation in the causal relationship between noncognitive education and urinary tract or kidney infection. This present study adhered to the guideline for Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology Using Mendelian Randomization (STROBE-MR).15
![]() |
Figure 1 The study design. |
GWAS Data Sources
The GWAS data sources for exposure, outcome, and mediating factors in this study have been summarized in Table 1. All data can be obtained from the IEU OpenGWAS project (https://gwas.mrcieu.ac.uk/) and GWAS Catalog (https://www.ebi.ac.uk/gwas/home) databases, with the corresponding GWAS ID provided in Table S1. The GWASs included in this study have all received ethical approval from the relevant institutional review board, obtained informed consent from the participants, and implemented strict quality control measures. This study received approval from the Medical Ethics Committee of Wuhan No.1 hospital (Approval No. [2024] 128). The potential for sample overlap in GWASs has been minimized to the greatest extent possible. Previous research indicates that when the instrumental variable intensity F-statistic exceeds 10, partial sample overlap has minimal impact on the analysis results.16 The linkage disequilibrium score regression (LDSC) was employed to analyze GWAS data on exposures and outcome, enabling the assessment of their heritability and genetic correlation without being influenced by sample overlap.17,18
![]() |
Table 1 Summary of the GWAS Data Used in the MR Analyses |
The genetic instrumental variables associated with intelligence were derived from a meta-analysis of 14 independent epidemiological cohorts comprising individuals of European ethnicities. Intelligence was primarily assessed using various neurocognitive tests. The intelligence GWAS had a sample size of 269,867 individuals and analyzed 7,276,181 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs).9 The education-related raw genetic instrumental variables were derived from a study on the years of schooling of 1,131,381 Europeans conducted by the Social Science Genetics Association. Similarly, the cognition-related raw genetic instrumental variables were obtained from a meta-analysis on cognitive performance scores of 257,841 Europeans conducted by the Cognitive Genomics Consortium and the UK Biobank.10 However, for this study, the genetic instrumental variables associated with noncognitive education and cognition were derived from a re-analysis of these studies using genomic structural equation modeling, participants from 23andMe were excluded as well. This is because previous research has shown that success in school and life depends not only on cognitive skills but also on noncognitive skills which have different genetic variations influencing educational attainment. The GWAS for noncognitive education included a sample size of 510,795 individuals and analyzed 7,305,956 SNPs, while the GWAS for cognition had a sample size of 257,700 individuals and analyzed an identical number of SNPs.11
The raw genetic instrumental variables for the outcome of urinary tract or kidney infection were derived from data obtained from the UK Biobank (https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/), a study that enrolled over half a million individuals aged 40 to 69 in the United Kingdom between 2006 and 2010. This comprehensive research initiative collected physical measurements, lifestyle information, medical history records, as well as conducted extensive analysis on blood, urine, and saliva samples. Additionally, long-term health follow-up was performed. However, the genetic instrumental variables utilized in this study were the data that underwent Firth logistic regression test for correction by a novel machine-learning method called REGENIE, based on UK Biobank data. This methodology is highly compatible with distributed computing frameworks and offers excellent accuracy and computational advantages. The GWAS for urinary tract or kidney infection included a sample size of 397,867 individuals and analyzed 11,037,343 SNPs.19
In this study, we examined 28 potential variables that may mediate the causal relationship between noncognitive education and urinary tract or kidney infection. These variables encompassed physical traits (body mass index [BMI],20 waist-to-hip ratio [WHR],22 waist circumference,22 body fat percentage [BF%],23 childhood obesity24), smoking and drinking habit (smoking initiation, age of smoking initiation, smoking heaviness, alcohol drinking, coffee intake),21 psycho-related traits (neuroticism,25 depressive symptoms,25 major depression,26 bipolar disorder,27 schizophrenia,28 anxiety, sleeping disorders), blood pressure (systolic blood pressure [SBP], diastolic blood pressure [DBP]),29 glucose related traits (fasting insulin, fasting glucose, two-hour glucose),30 lipid traits (triglycerides, total cholesterol, low-density lipoprotein cholesterol [LDL-C], high-density lipoprotein cholesterol [HDL-C]),31 and infective biomarkers (C-reactive protein,32 procalcitonin33). All biochemical parameters have been standardized in the initial GWAS study. The process of mediating factor analysis comprises three sequential steps, as shown in Figure 2. In the first step, we assessed the unidirectional causal effects of noncognitive education on candidate mediating variables; subsequently, we examined the unidirectional causal effects between candidate mediating variables and urinary tract or kidney infection; finally, we scrutinized the significant mediating influence of mediating variables in the causal relationship between noncognitive education and urinary tract or kidney infection.
![]() |
Figure 2 Process of mediator selection. |
MR Analysis Statistical Method
All MR analyses in this study were performed in R software (version 4.4.1; the R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria). The R packages utilized include “TwoSampleMR”, “ieugwasr”, “MRPRESSO”, “MendelianRandomization”, “MVMR”, “RMediation”, “ldscr” and “forestploter”. These packages can be obtained from GitHub or Comprehensive R Archive Network (CRAN).
The Selection of Instrumental Variables
The SNPs utilized in this study exhibited a robust association with exposure (P<5e-8) and successfully passed the linkage disequilibrium (LD) assessment (r² < 0.001; distance threshold > 10,000kb). The use of a strongly linked SNP (LD > 0.8) as a proxy SNP is recommended when no corresponding SNP is identified in the outcome GWAS data. The exposure and outcome data were harmonized to eliminate the palindrome structure, while the allele frequency of the palindrome was used for infer the positive chain alleles. Subsequently, the harmonized data underwent Steiger directivity test, which ensuring unidirectional causality between exposure and outcome by analyzing the strength of association between genetic instrumental variables and both exposure and outcome. Simultaneously, we computed the F-statistic to assess the validity of instrumental variables. Generally, an F-statistic value below 10 indicates weak instrumental variables.34 The presence of weak instrumental variables may result in false positive test outcomes. To obtain a more reliable estimate of the causal effect, we employed a random phenotypic covariance matrix and minimized heterogeneity using the Q statistic for correction testing in MVMR.35
UVMR and MVMR Analyses
The random effect model is analyzed in UVMR and MVMR using three methods: inverse variance weighted (IVW), weighted median, and MR-Egger. Among these methods, the IVW method is the primary calculation approach, requiring all SNPs to adhere to the three fundamental assumptions of MR and exhibiting the lowest tolerance for horizontal pleiotropy. The weighted median method can yield unbiased results when the MR assumptions is satisfied by more than 50% of valid SNPs.36 However, the MR-Egger method allows for 100% of SNPs to not satisfy the three assumptions of MR while demonstrating the highest tolerance for horizontal pleiotropy.37 We computed the Q statistic value to assess heterogeneity among the instrumental variables, although the random effects model can tolerate some degree of heterogeneity. We employed the intercept of MR-egger to evaluate horizontal pleiotropy in MR, and MR-PRESSO test was utilized to examine whether any outliers influence horizontal pleiotropy. Scatter plots and funnel plots were plotted for visualizing sensitivity analyses. P<0.05 indicates statistical significance. When the direction of the statistically significant results is consistent and the sensitivity analysis reveals no statistically significant horizontal pleiotropy in the absence of outliers, we consider the IVW method results as primary causal evidence. The odds ratio (OR), association coefficient β, proportion, and their 95% confidence intervals (CI) demonstrate the causal effect.
Two-Step Mediation MR Analyses
We employed two-step unidirectional UVMR to evaluate the mediating factors, thereby mitigating the bias associated with weak instrumental variables.38 By conducting UVMR on the causal relationship between noncognitive education and the mediating variables, we obtained the association coefficient β1. Subsequently, through further UVMR on the unidirectional causal relationship between the mediating variables and urinary tract or kidney infection, we derived the association coefficient β2, which represents the strength of mediation effect as β1 xβ2. The standard errors (SEs) of this mediation effect were estimated using the Delta method.39 Finally, we quantified the proportion of mediating effect as β1 xβ2 divided by total β from UVMR of noncognitive education on urinary tract or kidney infection.
Results
Total Causal Relationship Between Intelligence, Noncognitive Education, Cognition and Urinary Tract or Kidney Infection
The LDSC analysis revealed that the SNPs accounted for a genetic contribution of 0.178, 5.35e-02, 0.189, and 9.42e-03 to intelligence, noncognitive education, cognition, and urinary tract or kidney infection. Furthermore, the genetic correlation coefficients between intelligence, noncognitive education, cognition and urinary tract or kidney infection were found to be −0.338 (P=1.58e-14), −0.218 (P=2.45e-06), and −0.330 (P=2.78e-12) respectively. The results of LDSC analysis are presented in Table 2. After conducting LD assessment, removing palindrome structure through exposure and outcome harmonization, and performing the Steiger directionality test, a total of 114 SNPs showing significant correlation with intelligence, 44 SNPs associated with noncognitive education, and 114 SNPs related to cognition were identified. The intensity F-statistic for the genetic instrumental variables of intelligence was 40.942, while that for the genetic instrumental variables of noncognitive education was 38.317, and for the cognition genetic instrumental variables it was 44.115. All F-statistics exceeding 10 indicated a strong correlation between the selected SNPs and exposure. The results of the three exposures in UVMR analysis using IVW method were statistically significant and consistent with other statistically significant methods, indicating that for 1-SD unit increase in intelligence, the OR of urinary tract or kidney infection decreased to 0.841 (95% CI: 0.743 to 0.952). Additionally, each 1-SD higher of noncognitive education years was associated with a decrease in the OR to 0.920 (95% CI: 0.858 to 0.986), while each 1-SD increase of cognition was associated with a decrease to 0.892 (95% CI: 0.850 to 0.935). In the sensitivity analysis, significant heterogeneity was observed in the genetic instrumental variables of intelligence (Q:152.451, P=0.008) and noncognitive education (Q:60.538, P=0.040) for the outcome of urinary tract or kidney infection, while no significant heterogeneity was found in cognition (Q:128.083, P=0.157). There were no statistically significant differences in the intercepts of the MR-egger models, indicating an absence of significant horizontal pleiotropy in all exposures. The MR-PRESSO test did not identify any outliers among the genetic instrumental variables for the three exposure factors. The UVMR results are presented in Figure 3 and Table S2. Figure 4 displays scatter plots of three statistical analysis methods for each exposure, as well as funnel plots of heterogeneity analysis for each exposure.
![]() |
Table 2 Genetic Correlation Between Intelligence, Noncognitive Education, Cognition and Urinary Tract or Kidney Infection |
![]() |
Figure 3 Summary of results of UVMR and forest plot. |
Independent Causal Relationship Between Intelligence, Noncognitive Education, Cognition and Urinary Tract or Kidney Infection
The exposures were subjected to mutual adjustment analysis using IVW method primarily within the framework of MVMR. In the adjustment model for intelligence and noncognitive education, intelligence was significantly associated with 131 SNPs (F-statistic=27.174), while noncognitive education showed significant association with 31 SNPs (F-statistic=10.074)., these genetic instrumental variables exhibited a robust correlation with exposure. Each 1-SD increase in intelligence reduced the OR of urinary tract or kidney infection to 0.800 (95% CI: 0.716 to 0.895), while 1-SD higher in noncognitive education years reduced the OR to 0.929 (95% CI: 0.867 to 0.996). In the adjustment model for cognition and noncognitive education, cognition was found to be associated with 110 SNPs (F-statistic = 21.554), whereas noncognitive education showed association with 34 SNPs (F-statistic =10.644), genetic instrumental variables also demonstrated a strong relationship with exposure. Increasing cognition by 1-SD decreased the OR of urinary tract or kidney infection to 0.881 (95% CI: 0.838 to 0.925), while increasing 1-SD unit in noncognitive education years reduced it to 0.933 (95% CI: 0.874 to 0.995). These findings suggested that noncognitive education has an independent causal effect on urinary tract or kidney infection regardless of intelligence and cognition. However, in the adjustment model of intelligence and cognition, the genetic instrumental variables associated with them exhibited a weak correlation, and the IVW method results of MVMR analysis did not reach statistical significance. In the combined adjustment model encompassing intelligence, noncognitive education, and cognition, the genetic instrumental variables also displayed a weak correlation, and the results of MVMR analysis were not statistically significant. These findings suggest that intelligence or cognition represented non-independent causal effect on urinary tract or kidney infection. The genetic instrumental variables exhibited heterogeneity in the adjustment model for intelligence and noncognitive education (Q:209.989, P=0.003), but not in those adjustment for cognition and noncognitive education (Q:150.858, P=0.198). The intercept of the MR-Egger did not show significant statistic difference in these two models, indicating no apparent horizontal pleiotropy. Additionally, no outliers were detected by the MR-PRESSO test. The MVMR results are presented in Figure 5 and Table S3.
![]() |
Figure 5 Summary of IVW method results of MVMR and forest plot. |
The Effect of Mediators in the Causal Relationship Between Noncognitive Education and Urinary Tract or Kidney Infection
In the analysis of 28 candidate mediators, we initially performed a series of UVMR analyses with noncognitive education as exposure and the candidate mediators as outcomes. We excluded 14 mediators that did not show statistical significance, including waist circumference, childhood obesity, alcohol drinking, major depression, schizophrenia, anxiety, sleeping disorders, SBP, DBP, fasting glucose, two-hour glucose, total cholesterol, LDL-C and procalcitonin. Subsequent UVMR analyses was conducted with candidate mediators as exposures and urinary tract or kidney infection as outcome. We excluded 10 mediating factors that were not statistically significant, including WHR, BF%, neuroticism, depressive symptoms, bipolar disorder, age of smoking initiation, smoking heaviness, triglycerides, HDL-C and C-reactive protein. The remaining four mediators were analyzed for their mediating effects using the Delta method. However, coffee intake and fasting insulin did not reach statistical significance and were therefore excluded from further analysis. Finally, BMI and smoking initiation were selected as significant mediating factors in our study, their two-step UVMR results are presented in Tables 3 and 4. The two-step UVMR results for all potential mediators are summarized in Table S4 and Table S5. The final analysis revealed that BMI accounted for 12.1% (β: −0.010; 95% CI: −0.019 to −0.001; P=0.022) of the mediation effect in the causal relationship between noncognitive education and urinary tract or kidney infection, while smoking initiation accounted for 14.7% (β: −0.012; 95% CI: −0.023 to −0.001; P=0.030), The relevant results are presented in Table 5 and Table S6.
![]() |
Table 3 UVMR Assessing the Causal Association Between Noncognitive Education and Selected Mediator |
![]() |
Table 4 UVMR Assessing the Causal Association Between Selected Mediator and Urinary Tract or Kidney Infection |
![]() |
Table 5 MR Estimates of Selected Mediator in the Causal Association Between Noncognitive Education and Urinary Tract or Kidney Infection |
Discussion
Through this MR study, we have discovered that intelligence, noncognitive education and cognition exhibited negative causal effects on the occurrence of urinary tract or kidney infection. Furthermore, we have found that the causal effect of intelligence and cognition on urinary tract or kidney infection is not independent of each other, while the causal effect of noncognitive education on urinary tract or kidney infection is independent of intelligence and cognition. Specifically, for every 1-SD increase in noncognitive education years, there was an 8% decrease in the risk of urinary tract or kidney infection. Amongst 28 potential mediating factors examined in our study, both BMI and smoking initiation were identified as effective mediators with BMI accounting for a mediation role of 12.1% and smoking initiation accounting for a mediation role of 14.7%.
The previous GWAS have demonstrated bidirectional associations between intelligence, education, and cognition.9 However, the current study suggests that noncognitive skills encompassing motivation, curiosity, persistence, and self-control exert distinct influences on educational attainment compared to cognitive abilities. These noncognitive skills play a significant role in shaping future socioeconomic achievements, mental behaviors, and health outcomes.11 In a systematic review examining the relationship between noncognitive skills and academic achievement, it was found that higher levels of perceived stress are typically associated with lower academic performance, while greater conscientiousness, academic resilience, and perseverance tend to correlate with improved academic outcomes.40 A two-sample MVMR study revealed that both intelligence and education exert potential influences on individuals’ health and social well-being in later life, with education exhibiting a more pronounced direct impact on health compared to intelligence.41 Another MVMR study demonstrated the causal effect of education on blood pressure, independent of intelligence and cognition.42 Furthermore, our study also demonstrated the independent effects of noncognitive education on outcome related to urinary tract or kidney infection. Observational studies have demonstrated that enhancing educational attainment can significantly decrease the prevalence of UTIs,43,44 while surveys have revealed notable disparities in disease awareness and access to health education between women afflicted with UTIs and those without.45 Our study further expands upon these findings by elucidating the causal impact of noncognitive education on urinary tract or kidney infection through the lens of genetic variation. Previous research has demonstrated that cognitive dysfunction is linked to the dysregulation of multiple molecular signaling pathways in circulating exosomes and monocytes, which results in elevated serum levels of interleukin-6, TNF-alpha, and MCP-1/CCL2. This suggests a heightened inflammatory response during acute infection.8 Cross-sectional study has demonstrated a significant association between cognitive dysfunction and overactive bladder.46 Symptoms such as nocturnal urination, urgency incontinence, and urinary incontinence may elevate the risk of UTIs. Meta-analysis of twin studies has demonstrated a substantial heritability of cognitive function.47 While most cognitive tests, such as inductive and deductive reasoning abilities, along with the acquisition of declarative knowledge, contribute to the explanation of general intelligence, there exist specific assessments designed to evaluate distinct aspects of cognitive functioning. However, even polygenic scores fail to fully account for the complete heritability of intelligence.48 This observation may elucidate why intelligence and cognition exhibit a correlated rather than independent influence within this study. Through biogenetic studies, it has been discovered that noncognitive functional genes influencing educational attainment are extensively enriched in brain neurons, and the composition of brain tissue is similar to that of cognitive functional genes. However, disparities exist in terms of gray matter volume within the brain,11 which may elucidate the independent causal impact of noncognitive education observed in this study.
Additionally, we conducted an examination of the mediating role played by 28 potential factors in the causal relationship between noncognitive education and urinary tract or kidney infection. These factors encompassed physiological characteristics, lifestyle habits, mental state, basic vital signs, biochemical indicators, and other relevant aspects. Ultimately, the mediating effect of BMI and smoking initiation was found to be statistically significant in this study. In line with the findings of this study, another MR study involving 101,447 participants investigating the association between BMI and various infections revealed a consistent linear causal relationship specifically for UTIs.49 An observational study has also indicated that obesity independently contributes to the risk of UTIs particularly in males.50 This study extends the existing evidence by demonstrating that lower levels of noncognitive education are associated with an increased BMI, thereby elevating the risk of UTIs. However, no significant mediating effect was observed for other obesity-related traits such as waist circumference, WHR, BF%, and childhood obesity. Recent study has demonstrated male with similar BMI exhibit comparable urinary microbiome compositions. However, there are significant differences in the abundance of Corynebacterium, Staphylococcus, and Streptococcus among men classified as healthy weight, overweight, and obese.51 We also discovered that while smoking initiation played a significant role as a mediating factor, the heaviness of smoking and age of smoking initiation did not exhibit substantial mediating effects. This suggests that lower levels of noncognitive education are associated with an increased susceptibility to smoking, and once initiated, smoking can potentially contribute to urinary tract or kidney infection. GWAS on susceptibility to upper UTIs further corroborated these findings by identifying smoking as a potential risk factor.52 The combustion of cigarettes generates reactive oxygen species, and the resulting oxidative stress can trigger the activation of signaling cascades within epithelial cells. These signaling pathways encompass mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPK), nuclear factor κB (NF-κB), signal transducer and activator of transcription (STAT), and activator protein-1 (AP-1), which play crucial roles in the regulation of inflammation, cell cycle progression, and the expression of various genes.53 These mechanisms may account for the impact of smoking on UTIs. The mediating role of alcohol and coffee consumption was not found to be significant in our study, while the association between these factors and such infections still lacks convincing evidence from randomized controlled trials. In fact, the relationship between alcohol consumption and health outcomes remains subject to extensive debate.54
The GWAS identified genetic variants in noncognitive education that were significantly associated with an increased susceptibility to multiple psychiatric disorders,11 a finding which was further corroborated by this study. Utilizing a two-step MVMR mediation analysis, we observed a significant positive correlation between noncognitive education and bipolar disorder, while no statistically significant causal relationship was found between bipolar disorder and urinary tract or kidney infection. Similar to the findings of another MR study, we also observed a positive causal association between major depression and UTIs,55 while no statistically significant evidence was found for a causal relationship between noncognitive education and major depression. Although UVMR revealed a negative causal relationship between noncognitive education and depressive symptoms, the limited availability of valid genetic instrumental variables for depressive symptoms prevents further statistical elucidation of the causality underlying urinary tract or kidney infection. In future studies, expanding the study population and exploring additional genetic loci through GWAS for depressive symptoms will facilitate the identification of more genetic instrumental variables related to UTIs. Moreover, there was no significant causal association detected between genetic variations in noncognitive education and blood pressure or blood glucose levels. Regarding lipids, despite a negative correlation with triglycerides and a positive correlation with HDL-C, no significant causal relationship was observed between genetic variations in various lipid-related indicators and urinary tract or kidney infection. It is worth noting that C-reactive protein and procalcitonin are considered biomarkers of various infections, exhibiting high sensitivity to infection but low specificity to disease.56,57 This study revealed a negative causal effect of genetic variation in noncognitive education on C-reactive protein levels, while no significant one-way causal effect of C-reactive protein on urinary tract or kidney infection was observed. These findings further suggest that infection biomarkers do not act as mediators between noncognitive education and urinary tract or kidney infection.
This study presents novel evidence supporting the development of preventive strategies for urinary tract or kidney infection. In addition to cognitive limitations observed in diverse populations, educational attainment is more likely to improve post-learning, and enhancing noncognitive abilities through education is particularly crucial in reducing the risk of UTIs. Given the unequal distribution of educational resources across different environments, maintaining a healthy weight and quitting smoking play pivotal roles in improving UTIs.
The present study represents the first attempt to investigate the causal impact of genetic variation in intelligence, education, and cognition on the occurrence of urinary tract or kidney infection. The study possesses several strengths. Firstly, by utilizing education attainment as an exposure variable, we have employed data that effectively eliminates the influence of cognitive function on education level while emphasizing the role of noncognitive abilities in education outcomes. Secondly, the outcome data used in this study were not simply derived from the original data of UK Biobank. Instead, they underwent processing using a novel machine learning method called REGENIE that enables more efficient handling of quantitative and binary phenotypic whole-genome regression models. And the Firth correction was applied to effectively control Class 1 errors associated with binary traits. Thirdly, we conducted genetic correlation analysis between exposure traits and outcome using LDSC, and employed the F-statistic to validate the robust association between SNPs and exposure. We further validated heterogeneity and horizontal pleiotropy through sensitivity analysis. Additionally, we utilized a random phenotypic covariance matrix with minimized Q statistic to mitigate bias arising from weak instrumental variables in MVMR, thereby ensuring the reliability of our test results. The unidirectional mediating effect of 28 potential factors was assessed using SNPs identified via Steiger test. There are still several limitations in this study. Firstly, the samples included in the study all originate from European ancestry, however, there may exist genetic phenotypic variations among different geographical populations. Future studies should further validate the conclusions of this study by utilizing GWAS data from diverse populations to enhance generalizability. Secondly, although efforts have been made to select GWAS data from diverse research groups as correlation studies, it is inevitable that some sample overlap among these GWAS cannot be completely ruled out. Thirdly, persistent heterogeneity among SNPs might introduce certain biases in the test results. Fourthly, despite investigating 28 potential mediating factors, further exploration is required to identify other factors that potentially mediate the relationship between noncognitive education and urinary tract or kidney infection.
The present MR study revealed that enhancing intelligence, educational attainment, and cognition can effectively mitigate the risk of urinary tract or kidney infection. Notably, noncognitive abilities in education exert an independent causal influence on this relationship, while BMI and smoking initiation act as mediators. These findings provide valuable guidance for preventing urinary tract or kidney infection.
Abbreviations
BF%, Body fat percentage; BMI, Body mass index; DBP, Diastolic blood pressure; GWAS, Genome-wide association study; HDL-C, High-density lipoprotein cholesterol; IVW, Inverse variance weighted; LDL-C, Low-density lipoprotein cholesterol; MR, Mendelian randomization; MVMR, Multivariable Mendelian randomization; PRESSO, Pleiotropy residual sum and outlier; SBP, Systolic blood pressure; SNP, Single-nucleotide polymorphism; UTIs, Urinary tract infections; UVMR, Univariable Mendelian randomization; WHR, Waist-to-hip ratio.
Data Sharing Statement
The GWAS datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the IEU OpenGWAS project and GWAS Catalog, the corresponding GWAS ID are provided in Table S1. The R code is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request, and the contact way can get through by Email to [email protected].
Acknowledgments
Thanks for the support of the Department of Nephrology, Wuhan No. 1 Hospital. Reiterate our gratitude to the participants involved in the summary GWASs.
Funding
This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant No.82405332), the Natural Science Foundation of Hubei Province of China (Grant No.2023AFB505), the Wuhan Knowledge Innovation Special Project Fund (Grant No.2023020201020535).
Disclosure
All the authors declare that there is no conflict of interest in this work.
References
1. McLellan LK, Hunstad DA. Urinary tract infection: pathogenesis and Outlook. Trends mol med 2016;22:946–957. doi:10.1016/j.molmed.2016.09.003
2. Foxman B. The epidemiology of urinary tract infection. Nat Rev Urol. 2010;7:653–660. doi:10.1038/nrurol.2010.190
3. Kunin CM. Does kidney infection cause renal failure?. Annual Review Med. 1985;36:165–176. doi:10.1146/annurev.me.36.020185.001121
4. Deary IJ, Hill WD, Gale CR. Intelligence, health and death. Nat Human Behav. 2021;5:416–430. doi:10.1038/s41562-021-01078-9
5. Lövdén M, Fratiglioni L, Glymour MM, Lindenberger U, Tucker-Drob EM. Education and cognitive functioning across the life span. Psychol Sci Public Int. 2020;21:6–41. doi:10.1177/1529100620920576
6. Mackinnon N, Zammit S, Lewis G, Jones PB, Khandaker GM. Association between childhood infection, serum inflammatory markers and intelligence: findings from a population-based prospective birth cohort study. Epidemiol Infect. 2018;146:256–264. doi:10.1017/s0950268817002710
7. Johnson CY, Rocheleau CM, Howley MM, et al. Characteristics of women with urinary tract infection in pregnancy. J Women’s Health. 2021;30:1556–1564. doi:10.1089/jwh.2020.8946
8. Regueira P, Silva AR, Cardoso AL, et al. Peripheral inflammatory markers during an acute bacterial infection in older patients with and without cognitive dysfunction: a case control study. Brain Behavior Immunity Health. 2022;25:100503. doi:10.1016/j.bbih.2022.100503
9. Savage JE, Jansen PR, Stringer S. et.al. Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence. Nature Genet. 2018;50:912–919. doi:10.1038/s41588-018-0152-6
10. Lee JJ, Wedow R, Okbay A, et al. Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nature Genet. 2018;50:1112–1121. doi:10.1038/s41588-018-0147-3
11. Demange PA, Malanchini M, Mallard TT. et.al. Investigating the genetic architecture of noncognitive skills using GWAS-by-subtraction. Nat genetics 2021. 53:35–44. doi:10.1038/s41588-020-00754-2
12. Lawlor DA, Harbord RM, Sterne JA, Timpson N, Davey Smith G. Mendelian randomization: using genes as instruments for making causal inferences in epidemiology. Stat Med. 2008;27:1133–1163. doi:10.1002/sim.3034
13. Sanderson E, Davey Smith G, Windmeijer F, Bowden J. An examination of multivariable Mendelian randomization in the single-sample and two-sample summary data settings. Int J Epidemiol. 2019;48:713–727. doi:10.1093/ije/dyy262
14. Carter AR, Sanderson E, Hammerton G. Mendelian randomisation for mediation analysis: current methods and challenges for implementation. Eur j Epidemiol. 2021;36:465–478. doi:10.1007/s10654-021-00757-1
15. Skrivankova VW, Richmond RC, Woolf BAR. Strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology using Mendelian randomization: the STROBE-MR statement. JAMA. 2021;326:1614–1621. doi:10.1001/jama.2021.18236
16. Burgess S, Davies NM, Thompson SG. Bias due to participant overlap in two-sample Mendelian randomization. Genetic Epidemiology. 2016;40:597–608. doi:10.1002/gepi.21998
17. Bulik-Sullivan BK, Loh PR, Finucane HK. et.al. LD score regression distinguishes confounding from polygenicity in genome-wide association studies. Nature genetics. 2015. 47:291–295. doi:10.1038/ng.3211
18. Bulik-Sullivan B, Finucane HK, Anttila V. An atlas of genetic correlations across human diseases and traits. Nat genetics. 2015;47:1236–1241. doi:10.1038/ng.3406
19. Computationally efficient whole-genome regression for quantitative and binary traits. Nature genetics. 2021. 53:1097–1103. doi:10.1038/s41588-021-00870-7
20. Yengo L, Sidorenko J, Kemper KE. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for height and body mass index in ∼700000 individuals of European ancestry. Human Molecular Genetics. 2018;27:3641–3649. doi:10.1093/hmg/ddy271
21. Liu M, Jiang Y, Wedow R. Association studies of up to 1.2 million individuals yield new insights into the genetic etiology of tobacco and alcohol use. Nat Genet. 2019;51:237–244. doi:10.1038/s41588-018-0307-5
22. Shungin D, Winkler TW, Croteau-Chonka DC. New genetic loci link adipose and insulin biology to body fat distribution. Nature. 2015;518:187–196. doi:10.1038/nature14132
23. Lu Y, Day FR, Gustafsson S. New loci for body fat percentage reveal link between adiposity and cardiometabolic disease risk. Nat Commun. 2016;7:10495. doi:10.1038/ncomms10495
24. A genome-wide association meta-analysis identifies new childhood obesity loci. Nat genetics. 2012. 44:526–531. doi:10.1038/ng.2247
25. Okbay A, Baselmans BML, De Neve J-E. Genetic variants associated with subjective well-being, depressive symptoms, and neuroticism identified through genome-wide analyses. Nature Genet. 2016;48:624–633. doi:10.1038/ng.3552
26. Howard DM, Adams MJ, Clarke T-K. Genome-wide meta-analysis of depression identifies 102 independent variants and highlights the importance of the prefrontal brain regions. Nat Neurosci. 2019;22:343–352. doi:10.1038/s41593-018-0326-7
27. Genome-wide association study of more than 40,000 bipolar disorder cases provides new insights into the underlying biology. Nature Genet. 2021;53:817–829. doi:10.1038/s41588-021-00857-4
28. Trubetskoy V, Pardiñas AF, Qi T. Mapping genomic loci implicates genes and synaptic biology in schizophrenia. Nature. 2022;604:502–508. doi:10.1038/s41586-022-04434-5
29. Evangelou E, Warren HR, Mosen-Ansorena D. Genetic analysis of over 1 million people identifies 535 new loci associated with blood pressure traits. Nature Genet. 2018;50:1412–1425. doi:10.1038/s41588-018-0205-x
30. The trans-ancestral genomic architecture of glycemic traits. Nat genetics 2021. 53:840–860. doi:10.1038/s41588-021-00852-9
31. Willer CJ, et al. Discovery and refinement of loci associated with lipid levels. Nature genetics 2013;45:1274–1283. doi:10.1038/ng.2797
32. Ligthart S, Vaez A, Võsa U. Genome analyses of >200,000 individuals identify 58 loci for chronic inflammation and highlight pathways that link inflammation and complex disorders. Am J Hum Genet. 2018;103:691–706. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2018.09.009
33. Sun BB, Maranville JC, Peters JE. Genomic atlas of the human plasma proteome. Nature. 2018;558:73–79. doi:10.1038/s41586-018-0175-2
34. Using multiple genetic variants as instrumental variables for modifiable risk factors. Statistical methods med res 2012. 21:223–242. doi:10.1177/0962280210394459
35. Sanderson E, Spiller W, Bowden J. Testing and correcting for weak and pleiotropic instruments in two-sample multivariable Mendelian randomization. Stat Med. 2021;40:5434–5452. doi:10.1002/sim.9133
36. Bowden J, Davey Smith G, Haycock PC, Burgess S. Consistent estimation in Mendelian randomization with some invalid instruments using a weighted median estimator. Genetic Epidemiology. 2016;40:304–314. doi:10.1002/gepi.21965
37. Burgess S, Thompson SG. Interpreting findings from Mendelian randomization using the MR-Egger method. Eur j Epidemiol. 2017;32:377–389. doi:10.1007/s10654-017-0255-x
38. Relton CL, Davey Smith G. Two-step epigenetic Mendelian randomization: a strategy for establishing the causal role of epigenetic processes in pathways to disease. Int J Epidemiol. 2012;41:161–176. doi:10.1093/ije/dyr233
39. MacKinnon DP, Fairchild AJ, Fritz MSMA. Annual review of psychology. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007;58:593–614. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085542
40. Chisholm-Burns MA, Berg-Poppe P, Spivey CA, Karges-Brown J, Pithan A. Systematic review of noncognitive factors influence on health professions students’ academic performance. Adv health sci edu. 2021;26:1373–1445. doi:10.1007/s10459-021-10042-1
41. Multivariable two-sample Mendelian randomization estimates of the effects of intelligence and education on health. eLife. 2019;8. doi:10.7554/eLife.43990
42. Wang Y, Ye C, Kong L. Independent associations of education, intelligence, and cognition with hypertension and the mediating effects of cardiometabolic risk factors: a Mendelian randomization study. Hypertension. 2023;80:192–203. doi:10.1161/hypertensionaha.122.20286
43. Navarro A, Sison JM, Puno R. Reducing the incidence of pregnancy-related urinary tract infection by improving the knowledge and preventive practices of pregnant women. Eur J Obstet Gynecol Reprod Biol. 2019;241:88–93. doi:10.1016/j.ejogrb.2019.08.018
44. Eslami V, Sany SBT, Tehrani H, Ghavami V, Peyman N. Examining health literacy and self-efficacy levels and their association with preventive behaviors of urinary tract infection in Iranian pregnant women: across sectional study. BMC Women’s Health. 2023;23:258. doi:10.1186/s12905-023-02359-3
45. Gleicher S, Srinath M, Sebesta EM, Kaufman MR, Dmochowski RR, Stuart Reynolds W. Assessment and acquisition of knowledge regarding urinary tract infection among adult women in the United States. Urology. 2024;189:34–40. doi:10.1016/j.urology.2024.05.018
46. Park J, Chang Y, Choi HR. Overactive bladder and cognitive impairment in middle-aged women: a cross-sectional study. Maturitas. 2024;187:108042. doi:10.1016/j.maturitas.2024.108042
47. Polderman TJC, Benyamin B, De Leeuw CA. Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nature Genet. 2015;47:702–709. doi:10.1038/ng.3285
48. Genç E, Schlüter C, Fraenz C. Polygenic scores for cognitive abilities and their association with different aspects of general intelligence-a deep phenotyping approach. Molecular Neurobiology. 2021;58:4145–4156. doi:10.1007/s12035-021-02398-7
49. Winter-Jensen M, Afzal S, Jess T, Nordestgaard BG, Allin KH. Body mass index and risk of infections: a Mendelian randomization study of 101,447 individuals. Eur j Epidemiol. 2020;35:347–354. doi:10.1007/s10654-020-00630-7
50. Saliba W, Barnett-Griness O, Rennert G. The association between obesity and urinary tract infection. Eur J Internal Med. 2013;24:127–131. doi:10.1016/j.ejim.2012.11.006
51. Bowie KR, Garzotto M, Orwoll E, Karstens L. BMI and BPH correlate with urinary microbiome diversity and lower urinary tract symptoms in men. bioRxiv. 2023. doi:10.1101/2023.12.14.571758
52. Flatby HM, Ravi A, Liyanarachi KV, et al. A genome-wide association study of susceptibility to upper urinary tract infections. J Infect Dis. 2024:e1334–e1343. doi:10.1093/infdis/jiae231
53. Larsson SC, Burgess S. Appraising the causal role of smoking in multiple diseases: a systematic review and meta-analysis of Mendelian randomization studies. EBioMedicine. 2022;82:104154. doi:10.1016/j.ebiom.2022.104154
54. Visontay R, Mewton L, Sunderland M, Chapman C, Slade T. Is low-level alcohol consumption really health-protective? A critical review of approaches to promote causal inference and recent applications. Alcohol Clinical Exp Res. 2024;48:771–780. doi:10.1111/acer.15299
55. Shi L, Ren J, Jin K, Li J. Depression and risk of infectious diseases: a Mendelian randomization study. Transl Psychiatry. 2024;14:245. doi:10.1038/s41398-024-02950-8
56. Levinson T, Wasserman A. C-reactive protein velocity (CRPv) as a new biomarker for the early detection of acute infection/inflammation. Int J mol Sci. 2022;23:8100. doi:10.3390/ijms23158100
57. Paudel R, Dogra P, Montgomery-Yates AA, Coz Yataco A. Procalcitonin: a promising tool or just another overhyped test? Int J Med Sci. 2020;17:332–337. doi:10.7150/ijms.39367
© 2025 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The
full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php
and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution
- Non Commercial (unported, 3.0) License.
By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted
without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly
attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.