Back to Journals » Psychology Research and Behavior Management » Volume 17
How Health Anxiety is Associated with Perceived Risk of Reinfection Among COVID-19 Infected People After the Epidemic Control Measures Lifted in China: A Multiple Mediating and Multi-Group Analysis
Authors Zhao W, Ding N, Hu H, Tian W, He J , Du M, Zhao C, Zhang G
Received 27 August 2024
Accepted for publication 28 November 2024
Published 5 December 2024 Volume 2024:17 Pages 4157—4170
DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S493345
Checked for plagiarism Yes
Review by Single anonymous peer review
Peer reviewer comments 2
Editor who approved publication: Dr Bao-Liang Zhong
Wenqian Zhao,1,2,* Ningning Ding,2,* Haiyan Hu,2 Wenwen Tian,2 Jiankang He,2 Mingxuan Du,2 Chengjia Zhao,3 Guohua Zhang2,4,5
1Cixi Biomedical Research Institute, Wenzhou Medical University, Ningbo, 315302, People’s Republic of China; 2Department of Psychology, School of Mental Health, Wenzhou Medical University, Wenzhou, 325035, People’s Republic of China; 3School of Education, Renmin University of China, Beijing, 100872, People’s Republic of China; 4Zhejiang Provincial Clinical Research Center for Mental Health, The Affiliated Kangning Hospital of Wenzhou Medical University, Wenzhou, 325007, People’s Republic of China; 5Wenzhou Key Laboratory of Basic and Translational Research for Mental Disorders, Wenzhou, Zhejiang, 325035, People’s Republic of China
*These authors contributed equally to this work
Correspondence: Guohua Zhang, Email [email protected]
Background: After the lifting of epidemic control measures in China at the end of 2022, the number of COVID-19 infections has increased dramatically. Such an upsurge may intensify people’s health anxiety and raise concerns about the risk of reinfection. While numerous studies have shown the correlation between them during the COVID-19, research on their underlying mechanism remains limited.
Purpose: This study aims to test the relationship and mechanism between health anxiety and perceived risk of reinfection among COVID-19 infected people from different occupational backgrounds.
Methods: A cross-sectional study was conducted from December 24th to 28th, 2022, after the epidemic control measures were lifted. A total of 1122 Chinese individuals were infected with COVID-19 (67.91% females, Mage = 28.13) completed measures of health anxiety, perceived risk of reinfection, fear of COVID-19, online health information seeking (OHIS), and COVID-19 information fear of missing out (FOMO). Data were analyzed using multiple-mediation model and multi-group analysis.
Results: Health anxiety, perceived risk of reinfection, fear of COVID-19, OHIS and COVID-19 information FOMO were positively and significantly related to each other (r ranged from 0.25 to 0.77, all p < 0.001). Fear of COVID-19 (β = 0.27, p < 0.01), OHIS (β = 0.10, p < 0.001), and COVID-19 information FOMO (β = − 0.05, p < 0.01) fully mediated the relationship between health anxiety and perceived risk of reinfection, the path of chaining mediation is also significant. Discrepancies in mediation models surfaced across the three types of work groups.
Conclusion: Findings suggest fear of COVID-19, OHIS and COVID-19 information FOMO may play significant mediating roles in explaining the relationship between health anxiety and perceived risk of reinfection. Interventions to reduce the perceived risk of reinfection should aim to improve the quality of online health information and individuals’ online health literacy and reduce fear of COVID-19 during and after the pandemic.
Keywords: health anxiety, perceived risk of reinfection, fear of COVID-19, COVID-19 information FOMO, online health information seeking, people infected with COVID-19
Introduction
Since the outbreak of the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) in January 2020,1 the Chinese government had taken a series of measures, such as travel restrictions, community controls, and self-isolation, to prevent and control the COVID-19 pandemic.2 With the virus proving less lethal over time, the Chinese government announced 10 new measures to lift numerous COVID-19 restrictions on December 7, 2022,3 which led to a sharp increase in the number of infections in a short time and the infection rate peaked at 29.2% on December 25.4 Given the high prevalence and potential of reinfection, it is meaningful to understand the perceived risk of reinfection in infected COVID-19 people, as well as the factors that influence it.
Health anxiety (HA) is a fear of having or developing diseases, ranging from mild worry to excessive anxiety.5,6 A meta-analysis indicated that over 30% of people have experienced COVID-19 anxiety.7 A research which focused on the working population have shown that COVID-19 related health anxiety detrimentally impacted workplace functioning, hindering goal progression.8 China’s COVID-19 vaccination rate was over 90% at the time of the survey,4 and infected individuals develop immunity to the virus. However, as the virus mutates, infected individuals remain at risk of reinfection. Thus, it is necessary to examine how health anxiety affects the perceived risk of reinfection with COVID-19 among infected people.
Perceived risk of infection entails an individual’s subjective evaluation of the severity and vulnerability to infection.9,10 A wide variety of studies have indicated that higher infection risk perceptions were related to increased anxiety.11 The association between anxiety and COVID-19 risk perception has been supported in different countries, such as Swiss,12 Nigeria,13 and China;14 intense COVID-19 anxiety was also associated with high infection risk perception.15 We define the perceived risk of reinfection as infected people’s subjective perception and judgment of reinfection. Research showed that concern about reinfection with COVID-19 were associated with anxiety symptoms in German university students.16 However, limited studies have discussed the relationship between health anxiety and the perceived risk of reinfection in China. Therefore, it is essential to explore the relationship between health anxiety and perceived risk of reinfection.
Fear is an instinctive response to threats, danger, and risks. Fear of COVID-19 has emerged as a primary determinant of escalated negative emotions, including anxiety, depression, stress, and mental health disorders.17–19 There is a strong link between individual anxiety about health and fear of COVID-19. This fear can also foster stigmatization, such as the perception that healthcare workers during a pandemic may spread the disease.20 Since the fear of COVID-19 correlates with the perception of COVID-19 risk,21 which indirectly states that fear of COVID-19 is associated to the perceived risk of reinfection. To ease panic and anxiety emotions, people often resort to online platforms during public health emergencies, including online health information seeking (OHIS),22 which refers to all actions taken by individuals to acquire, clarify, or comprehend health-related knowledge, recognize health risks, and prevent diseases in response to particular circumstances or events.23 Meantime, people may keep searching for related news and read information about COVID-19 with a fear of missing out (FOMO) on social media, which wants to get a better grasp of COVID-19 and reduce their risk of infection.24 COVID-19 information FOMO is the anxiety an individual experiences regarding information control as COVID-19 spreads.25 With the diversification of media, online platforms have become the prime means of search for health information.26 But there is no denying that while online platforms provide a wealth of information, there are also some erroneous information and rumors, which influences users’ health information seeking behavior and even results in negative results such as disruptive behaviors, information overload or avoidance, distrust and distress.27 This negative information may interfere with an individual’s judgement of risk perception. Therefore, it is important to identify potential mediating factors between health anxiety and perceived risk of reinfection.
According to the ripple effect, far from danger or risk can decrease fear.28 But some studies have found the contrary results, that is residents who are closer to the risk center have lower levels of anxiety and worry, this phenomenon is called the psychological typhoon eye (PTE) effect.29 Some studies about the COVID-19 have supported the ripple effect,30,31 while others have supported the typhoon eye effect.32,33 Researches have shown that increased viral exposure time was one of the risk factors for COVID-19 infection.34 In other words, the more frequent the face-to-face contact with people, the higher the infection risk. Therefore, based on the theories of the typhoon eye and ripple effects, it becomes imperative to evaluate the differences in the relationship between health anxiety, perceived risk of reinfection, fear of COVID-19, online health information seeking and COVID-19 information FOMO in terms of work types.
Consequently, this study tests the relationship and mechanism between health anxiety and perceived risk of reinfection among COVID-19 infected people from different work types. Based on previous studies, we put forward the following hypotheses:
H1: Health anxiety would have a positive correlation with the perceived risk of reinfection. H2a: Fear of COVID-19 would mediate the relationship between health anxiety and the perceived risk of reinfection. H2b: Online health information seeking would mediate the relationship between health anxiety and perceived risk of reinfection. H2c: COVID-19 information FOMO would mediate the relationship between health anxiety and perceived risk of reinfection. H3: Substantial differences between work types in the relationships among health anxiety, fear of COVID-19, online health information seeking, COVID-19 information FOMO and perceived risk of reinfection.
Method
Participants and Procedure
After the epidemic control measures were lifted, a cross-sectional survey was conducted from December 24th to 28th, 2022, covering 25 provinces and four municipalities in China. Participants who self-reported being infected with COVID-19 were recruited through convenient sampling. The data was collected through an online survey link of Wenjuanxing (www.wjx.cn), a professional online survey platform in China. The research team sent the online survey questionnaire (see Supplementary Material 1) on the social media platform (ie, WeChat). No personal information (eg, names and contact information) was collected in this survey. The survey was self-administered and required approximately 10–15 minutes to complete. It was stated in the invitation and online questionnaire that participation was anonymous, voluntary and confidential. Responding to the questionnaire indicated informed consent had been given. Participants received 5 RMB (0.68 USD) after their participation. The inclusion criteria included that individuals infected with COVID-19 who could finish the questionnaire and submit voluntarily, and the exclusion criteria included that those who were not infected with COVID-19 and did not want to participant into this study. Finally, a total of 1133 participants completed the questionnaire, with 11 invalid responses excluded (ie, answering time is less than 120 seconds). Ethical approval was obtained from the ethics committee of the corresponding author’s university.
Measures
Background Variables
The study collected demographic information about the participants, including age, gender, educational level, types of work requiring high/medium/low frequency of face-to-face contact with people, residence, current infection status, and typical symptoms of infection. Additionally, participants self-reported their current physical and mental health status using a scale (“How do you rate your current physical / mental health status?”) from 1 (poor) to 5 (excellent).
Health Anxiety
The health anxiety scale was used to assess individuals’ concerns about their health status during the pandemic.35 Participants were asked to indicate to what extent they experienced (1) worry about their health, (2) fear of becoming sick, and (3) perceived concerns about their health from their family and friends. The scale included three items, rated on a seven-Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree), with higher scores indicating a higher level of health anxiety. In this study, the Cronbach’s α of the scale was 0.85.
Online Health Information Seeking
The online health information seeking behavior scale was used to assess online health information seeking behavior in the context of an epidemic.35 The scale consists of four items (eg, “During the epidemic, I always proactively search for relevant health information online when encountering health problems”.), evaluated on a seven-Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree), with higher scores indicating more searching for health information through the internet. The Cronbach’s α of the scale was 0.94.
Perceived Risk of Reinfection with COVID-19
Considering that all participants in this study had all experienced a COVID-19 infection, we adapted the COVID-19 Risk Perception Scale from Cui36 to assess individuals’ perceived risk of reinfection with COVID-19. The scale consists of nine items (eg, “I have a high possibility of reinfection”.) and three dimensions (ie, severity, susceptibility, and controllability). Each item is rated on a scale of 1 to 5, with higher scores indicating a greater perceived risk of reinfection with COVID-19. In this study, the Cronbach’s α for the total scale and the three subscales were 0.94, 0.88, 0.92, and 0.83, respectively.
COVID-19 Information FOMO
We evaluated the FOMO concerning COVID-19 information using the COVID-19 Information FOMO Scale (CIFS).25 The scale consists of six questions (eg, “Once I use my mobile phone, I will subconsciously search for information about COVID-19”.), with each item scored on a 4-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 4 = strongly agree). In this study, the Cronbach’s α of the scale was 0.91.
Fear of COVID-19
The study employed the Fear of COVID-19 Scale (FCV-19S-C) in its Chinese version to evaluate participants’ perceptions of COVID-19.37 The scale comprises of 7 items (eg, “I am very afraid of COVID-19”.), each rated on a 5-point Likert scale, with higher scores indicating greater fear of COVID-19. In this study, the Cronbach’s α of the scale was 0.91.
Statistical Analysis
Data analysis was conducted using SPSS 25.0 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA) and AMOS 23.0 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA). Categorical variables were presented as frequencies and percentages (%), while continuous variables were described using means (M) and standard deviation (SD). Pearson correlations were used to assess associations between measured variables. Chi-square test, ANOVA were employed to test the significance of between-group differences. The structural equation modeling (SEM) approach was employed to examine mediation models utilizing maximum likelihood estimation. Multi-group analysis was conducted to determine whether SEMs differed across three groups. The study tested for direct, indirect, and total effects using 5000 bootstrapped samples. Effect estimates and bias-corrected 95% confidence intervals (CI) were calculated. Indices of good model fit included the Root Means Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) <0.08 (<0.1 is acceptable), the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), goodness-of-fit index (GFI), Normed Fit Index (NFI) and the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) >0.95.38,39 A two-sided p below 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Results
Common Method Bias Testing
Common method bias testing was performed. According to Harman’s single-factor test, 11 principal components were extracted without rotation, and the explanatory rate of the total variance variation of the first was 23.06%, lower than the critical value of 40.0%.40 Consequently, the data exhibited no serious common method bias in the data.
Preliminary Analyses
Among the 1122 participants analyzed in the analysis, 360 were male (32.09%) and 762 were female (67.91%); 629 were infected and 493 had recovered from the infection. The age of the participants ranged from 14 to 70 years (M = 28.13, SD = 10.05). Participants’ demographic information is shown in Table 1.
![]() |
Table 1 Demographics and Background Characteristics of the Participants |
As shown in Table 2, all measured variables had significant and positive correlations (r ranged from 0.25 to 0.77, all p < 0.001).
![]() |
Table 2 Correlations Between the Measured Variables |
Mentality of Knowing That People Have Been Infected
As Table 1 shows, when people found that he/she has already been infected, 58.38% of people were relaxed or calm, 19.61% of people were numb or insensible, and 13.90% of people were fearful or anxious. The result of the chi-square test showed that there were significant differences between the different mentalities (χ2(3) = 696.33, p < 0.001). When family member(s) knew that they had been infected, 48.93% of people were relaxed or calm, 29.32% of people were fearful or anxious, and 10.52% of people were numb or insensible. The result of the chi-square test showed that there were significant differences between the different mentalities (χ2(4) = 804.85, p < 0.001).
Comparison of Differences Between Types of Work in Research Variables
Table 3 delineates the differences among different groups in measured variables. In terms of health anxiety (F (2, 1119) = 3.21, p < 0.05), fear of COVID-19 (F (2, 1119) = 5.19, p < 0.01), online health information seeking (F (2, 1119) = 4.65, p < 0.05), and perceived risk of reinfection (F (2, 1119) = 7.46, p < 0.001), the results of ANOVA showed that there were significant differences among the three groups. In terms of COVID-19 information FOMO, the results of ANOVA showed that there were no significant differences among the three groups (F (2, 1119) = 1.16, p = 0.313).
![]() |
Table 3 Differences Between Types of Work in Research Variables |
Structural Equation Modelling
First, linear regression analyses indicated that health anxiety was directly and positively associated with the perceived risk of reinfection (β = 0.36, t = 13.015, p < 0.001). Therefore, H1 was supported. Second, the hypothetical mediation model in our study achieved a good model fit (χ2/df = 6.053, CFI = 0.988, GFI = 0.985, NFI = 0.986, TLI = 0.975, RMSEA = 0.067). The mediating effects of online health information seeking, fear of COVID-19 and COVID-19 information FOMO by bootstrap are shown in Figure 1 and Table 4. The bootstrapped 95% confidence interval for the standardized indirect effect does not encompass zero. This indicates statistically significant indirect pathways from health anxiety to the perceived risk of reinfection via online health information-seeking, fear of COVID-19, and COVID-19 information FOMO. Consequently, hypotheses H2a, H2b, and H2c are confirmed. Third, health anxiety was not directly related to the perceived risk of reinfection (β = 0.09, 95% CI = −0.10 to 0.18). Thus, the complete mediation effects were demonstrated.
![]() |
Table 4 The Bootstrap Confidence Interval and Effect Size of the Mediation Model |
Multi-Group Analysis
The multi-group analysis was performed to examine whether structural equation modeling differed between high frequency, medium frequency, and low-frequency groups. First, the groups are modeled. The model results all show good fit indices and satisfy the prerequisites for multi-group analysis: High frequency: χ2/df=2.562, CFI=0.993, GFI=0.985, NFI=0.988, TLI=0.983, RMSEA=0.06 (see Figure 2); Medium frequency: χ2/df=4.579, CFI=0.980, GFI=0.977, NFI=0.974, TLI=0.952, RMSEA=0.085 (see Figure 3); Low frequency: χ2/df=1.120, CFI=0.998, GFI=0.985, NFI=0.985, TLI=0.996, RMSEA=0.026 (see Figure 4).
![]() |
Figure 2 The structural equation model for mediation effect in high frequency group (n = 441); ⁎⁎⁎ p < 0.001. |
![]() |
Figure 3 The structural equation model for mediation effect in medium frequency group (n = 496); ⁎ p < 0.05; ⁎⁎ p < 0.01; ⁎⁎⁎ p < 0.001. |
![]() |
Figure 4 The structural equation model for mediation effect in low frequency group (n = 185); ⁎⁎⁎ p < 0.001. |
Second, it was essential to verify the measurement invariance of measured variables across the three groups. The model fits are shown in Table 5. Although the Chi-square test of structural covariances and measurement residuals were significant (p < 0.05), the absolute values of ΔNFI, ΔIFI, ΔRFI, and ΔTLI were below 0.05. That is, the assumption of measurement invariance is accepted, thus allowing this measurement model to be further analyzed.
![]() |
Table 5 The Measurement Invariance of the Scale Between Different Groups |
Third, good model fit indices for the unconstrained baseline model, and there is a significant difference between the structural weights constrained model and the measurement weights constrained model. This indicates the mediation model’s general applicability across all groups, but with at least one path coefficient varying significantly among them (Table 6).
![]() |
Table 6 The Fitting Index of the Multiple-Group Analysis Model |
Finally, the critical rate of difference (CRD) was used as an indicator to analyze the difference in path coefficients between the different groups (Table 7). A significant difference was considered if the absolute values of CRD exceeded 1.96 (p < 0.05). The results show that there are two paths differed between the groups: (path 1) Health anxiety→ Online health information seeking; (path 2) COVID-19 information FOMO → Fear of COVID-19. In Path 1, the high-frequency group is significantly larger than the medium frequency group (CRD = −2.40, p < 0.05); and in path 2, the high-frequency group is significantly larger than both the medium (CRD = −2.64, p < 0.05) and low-frequency group (CRD = −2.39, p < 0.05). Thus, H3 was supported.
![]() |
Table 7 The Critical Ratios of Path Coefficient in the Multiple-Group Analysis Model |
Discussion
As noted above, this research investigated the mentality state of finding out that they or their family members have been infected COVID-19 in China. In addition, a structural equation model was constructed to analyze the mechanisms underlying the relationship between health anxiety and perceived risk of reinfection among COVID-19 infected people from different occupational backgrounds. First, this study found that people were more worried about family members getting infected than themselves. Second, people far from the center of risk had higher levels of health anxiety and fear of COVID-19 than those in the center of risk; however, people in the center of risk had a higher perceived risk of reinfection than those far from the center of risk. Additionally, Fear of COVID-19, OHIS and COVID-19 information FOMO played a mediation role in the relationship between health anxiety and the perceived risk of reinfection.
Mentality That People Found They or Their Family Members Have Been Infected
The results showed that a small number of people (19.61%) were numb or insensitive to being infected with COVID-19, and even more than half (58.38%) showed a relaxed or calm psychological state after discovering their infection with COVID-19. People had been exposed to the risk of COVID-19 infection for up to 3 years before being infected, and one follow-up study found that both anxiety and fear of COVID-19 diminished over time.15,41,42 In addition, at the beginning of the COVID-19 epidemic, infected individuals were at higher risk of developing psychological symptoms.43 As COVID-19 mutates and the Omicron strain becomes a predominantly prevalent strain, the pathogenicity decreases, people become more experienced in treating the disease and face lower health risks after infection. For these reasons, it is likely that most people who are infected with COVID-19 do not feel fear but show calmness or even relaxation. In contrast to their infection, the number of people who felt fearful or anxious when their family members were infected with COVID-19 was 29.32%. The participants in this study were predominantly young and middle-aged (age: 28.13 ± 10.05), with a significant number having children or elderly individuals in their household. It is worth noting that both children and the elderly, as well as those with pre-existing health conditions, are at an increased risk of severe outcomes post-COVID-19 infection.8 Therefore, people are more worried about their family members’ health status.
Mediations Between Health Anxiety and Perceived Risk of Reinfection
H1 was supported by the results and suggested that high levels of health anxiety is correlated with an augmented perceived risk of COVID-19 reinfection. In previous studies, other scholars have also found that health anxiety is not only related to individuals’ perceived risk of the coronavirus disease during the COVID-19 pandemic,44,45 but also to the perceived risk of reinfection after the lifting of epidemic control measures.
The structural equation modeling analysis indicated that all the mediating effects were significant; H2a-c was supported. This indicates that online health information seeking, fear of COVID-19, and COVID-19 information FOMO could be significant mediators concerning health anxiety and the perceived risk of reinfection. Health anxiety could compel individuals to seek health-related information online.46 Although having comprehensive knowledge about COVID-19 could mitigate fear and bias, the media’s coverage of emerging virus variants can potentially enhance the perceived risk of reinfection. The greater an individual’s anxiety about their health, the higher the level of fear of COVID-19 and the greater the fear that one will miss out on information about COVID-19.44,45 Fear of COVID-19 increases individuals’ perceived risk of reinfection, individuals will search for knowledge online for fear of missing out on information related to COVID-19, and more excellent knowledge of vaccination will lead them to take more vaccination measures,47 which helps to reduce their likelihood of reinfection. It’s crucial to note that, given the cross-sectional design of this study, these results are exploratory in nature and do not imply causation, but they can be used as reference knowledge in the management of responses to public health emergencies. For example, in a significant public health event, regulation of online information is necessary, and more correct knowledge facilitates people to take reasonable measures in response to an outbreak.48
Differences in Work Types
The findings lend partial support to H3; a “psychological typhoon eye (PTE)” effect was found in cross-group comparisons of health anxiety and fear of COVID-19, ie, there was no correspondence between objective risk and people’s subjective fears. Prior research has yielded comparable findings. During the peak of SARS in 2003, researchers found that the closer people were to high-risk times or high-risk locations, the calmer their psychological state was.49 In the wake of the COVID-19 crisis, adults in Brazil who go to work less often and have less contact with people have higher levels of psychological distress and are more vulnerable.50 In Pakistan, the level of anxiety and distress among adults in small families is found to increase in direct proportion to the distance from the epicenter.51 After the lockdown measures were lifted, individuals with frequent face-to-face interactions exhibited reduced health anxiety and diminished fear of COVID-19, this is also consistent with the findings of prior studies.52 The Individual Experience Theory (REF) provides a plausible explanation for these observations. Individuals in the risk-centered zone rely on experiencing negative events to perceive risk, which is an empirical perception; however, individuals in the non-risk-centered zone rely on a variety of media messages or narratives from others to perceive risk. Empirical perceptions can downplay the likelihood of rare events.53–56 Due to the spread of the COVID-19 vaccine in China (more than 90% of the population is vaccinated) and the diminishing pathogenicity of the prevalent strains,4 the emergence of critically ill individuals has become a small probability event. Consequently, individuals at risk centers perceive severe infections as unlikely, thus experiencing reduced health anxiety and fear compared to those outside risk centers. In addition, the results of the study showed that individuals outside risk centers tend to seek health information more actively, driven by heightened concerns about the virus, prompting them to gather protective information.57
For the perceived risk of reinfection with COVID-19, our results show a “ripple effect”, whereby people’s perceived risk of reinfection corresponds to the objective risk, ie, the closer they are to the center of the risk, the higher the risk that they believe they will be reinfected. This perception is clearly objective and rational, because the higher the exposure to risk, the higher the likelihood of reinfection. In all three groups, the high-frequency group exhibited a stronger correlation between health anxiety and OHIS than the medium frequency group. Additionally, the impact of COVID-19 information FOMO on fear of COVID-19 was more pronounced in the high-frequency group than in the low-frequency group. Despite the differences in the two pathways, the model structure did not change in the three groups as a result (ΔCFI < 0.01, ΔTLI < 0.005). These differences might stem from group size disparities. We can only assume that the mechanisms of perceived risk of reinfection are similar across groups, but that the levels of these factors are different. The mental health of non-risk-centered groups, who experience more anxiety and fear than that of risk-centered groups, should also receive attention during significant health events.
Strengths and Limitations
Our study investigated the psychological state of the public and changes in perceptions of COVID-19 after the Chinese government transitioned from a “Category B-A” policy to a “Category B-B” policy for epidemics, which is significant to provide a new reference for people facing public health events. Although the confusion and uncertainty of the pandemic have been greatly reduced in the post-COVID-19 era, the findings of this study will serve as important foundational data for the prevention and control of future infectious diseases. For instance, the “psychological typhoon eye (PTE)” effect posits that resources for psychological assistance should not be concentrated in crisis centers. This is because the anxiety and fear of groups outside the centers also require psychological guidance and intervention. To reduce the panic of the masses, the government should build a good online information environment. Our results also reveal more about the impact of psychological factors on perceived risk of reinfection from different occupational background groups, which should be further examined in future research and practices.
This study also has several limitations that should be noted in future research. First, the convenient sampling method had a notable impact on the demographics of the sample. The majority (67.9%) were females, and 94.3% held at least a bachelor’s degree, and fewer older people and minors were included, which limits the generalization of the results to other samples. Nevertheless, the study’s sample size is sufficiently large and encompasses a diverse geographical range, ensuring the sample’s representativeness and the reliability of the findings. Second, the findings from a cross-sectional study design cannot be used to draw causal inferences, so interpretation of the results should be interpreted with caution. Third, the sources of the data were all self-reported by the participants, which may be subject to common method bias. Fourth, there were high correlation coefficients between variables (r > 0.7), and the multicollinearity of the data may affect the results. However, the structural equation model used in this study was better able to tolerate multicollinearity.58 Finally, the study did not collect information on biological tests (eg, nucleic acid or antigen results) from the subjects. There were significant difficulties in collecting these results when the survey was conducted.
Conclusions
Our study has found that people are more worried about family members getting infected than themselves. Moreover, the findings suggest that people far from the center of risk have higher levels of health anxiety and fear of COVID-19, supporting the “psychological typhoon eye (PTE)” hypothesis; people in the center of risk have a higher perceived risk of reinfection, supporting the “ripple effect” hypothesis. This guides us to adopt targeted coping strategies for different risk zones peoples. Fear of COVID-19, OHIS and COVID-19 information FOMO play mediating roles in the relationship between health anxiety and the perceived risk of reinfection. Therefore, interventions for reducing perceived risk of reinfection should aim at improving the quality of online health information and individuals’ online health literacy and reducing fear of COVID-19 during and after the pandemic. To provide more theoretical support for crisis intervention, future research could track anxiety and fear in groups following a pandemic.
Data Sharing Statement
The datasets are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Ethics Approval and Consent to Participate
Ethical approval was granted by the Ethics Committee of Wenzhou Medical University [Grant No. 2022-028]. Prior to participating in the investigation, the participants were fully informed about it, and their completion and submission of the questionnaire are regarded as informed consent. In this study, all participants were voluntary, the questionnaire was anonymous, and we also used the non-identification code to ensure the confidentiality of information. This study was conducted following the Declaration of Helsinki.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the participants for their participation.
Funding
This research was funded by the Zhejiang Province Educational Science Planning Project in 2023 [Grant No. 2023SCG210].
Disclosure
The authors declare that they have no competing interests in this work.
References
1. World Health Organization. Number of COVID-19 cases reported to WHO. Available from: https://covid19.who.int/data.
2. Cheng S, Zhao Y, Kaminga AC, Zhang X, Xu H. China’s fight against COVID-19: what we have done and what we should do next? Front Public Health. 2022;10:548056. doi:10.3389/fpubh.2022.548056
3. Further optimize and implement the 10 latest measures for epidemic prevention and control.
4. Epidemic situation of novel coronavirus infection in China.
5. Ferguson E. A taxometric analysis of health anxiety. Psychol Med. 2009;39(2):277–285. doi:10.1017/S0033291708003322
6. Rachman S. Health anxiety disorders: a cognitive construal. Behav Res Ther. 2012;50(7–8):502–512. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2012.05.001
7. Salari N, Hosseinian-Far A, Jalali R, et al. Prevalence of stress, anxiety, depression among the general population during the COVID-19 pandemic: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Glob Health. 2020;16(1):57. doi:10.1186/s12992-020-00589-w
8. Trougakos JP, Chawla N, McCarthy JM. Working in a pandemic: exploring the impact of COVID-19 health anxiety on work, family, and health outcomes. J Appl Psychol. 2020;105(11):1234–1245. doi:10.1037/apl0000739
9. Choi C, Oh KR, Jun MG. COVID-19 obesity: differences in infection risk perception, obesity stress, depression, and intention to participate in leisure sports based on weight change. Healthcare. 2023;11(4):526. doi:10.3390/healthcare11040526
10. Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E, MacGregor DG. Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Anal. 2004;24(2):311–322. doi:10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00433.x
11. Choi EPH, Hui BPH, Wan EYF. Depression and Anxiety in Hong Kong during COVID-19. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(10):3740. doi:10.3390/ijerph17103740
12. Dratva J, Zysset A, Schlatter N, Von Wyl A, Huber M, Volken T. Swiss university students’ risk perception and general anxiety during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(20):7433. doi:10.3390/ijerph17207433
13. Oyetunji TP, Ogunmola OA, Oyelakin TT, Olorunsogbon OF, Ajayi FO. COVID-19-related risk perception, anxiety and protective behaviours among Nigerian adults: a cross-sectional study. J Public Health. 2023;31(3):479–487. doi:10.1007/s10389-021-01502-4
14. Liu M, Zhang H, Huang H. Media exposure to COVID-19 information, risk perception, social and geographical proximity, and self-rated anxiety in China. BMC Public Health. 2020;20(1):1649. doi:10.1186/s12889-020-09761-8
15. Costa MDA, Kristensen CH, Dreher CB, Manfro GG, Salum GA. Habituating to pandemic anxiety: temporal trends of COVID-19 anxiety over sixteen months of COVID-19. J Affect Disord. 2022;313:32–35. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2022.06.077
16. Heumann E, Helmer SM, Busse H, et al. Depressive and anxiety symptoms among university students during the later stages of the COVID-19 pandemic in Germany - Results from the COVID 19 German Student Well-being Study (C19 GSWS). Front Public Health. 2024;12:1459501. doi:10.3389/fpubh.2024.1459501
17. Li G, Liu H, Qiu C, Tang W. Fear of COVID-19, prolonged smartphone use, sleep disturbances, and depression in the time of COVID-19: a nation-wide survey. Front Psychiatry. 2022;13:971800. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2022.971800
18. Schweda A, Weismüller B, Bäuerle A, et al. Phenotyping mental health: age, community size, and depression differently modulate COVID-19-related fear and generalized anxiety. Compr Psychiatry. 2021;104:152218. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2020.152218
19. Zhao Y, Yu Y, Zhao R, et al. Association between fear of COVID-19 and hoarding behavior during the outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic: the mediating role of mental health status. Front Psychol. 2022;13:996486. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2022.996486
20. Taylor S, Landry CA, Rachor GS, Paluszek MM, Asmundson GJG. Fear and avoidance of healthcare workers: an important, under-recognized form of stigmatization during the COVID-19 pandemic. J Anxiety Disord. 2020;75:102289. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102289
21. Capone V, Donizzetti AR, Park MSA. Validation and psychometric evaluation of the COVID-19 risk perception scale (CoRP): a new brief scale to measure individuals’ risk perception. Int J Ment Health Addict. 2023;21(3):1320–1333. doi:10.1007/s11469-021-00660-6
22. Tausczik Y, Faasse K, Pennebaker JW, Petrie KJ. Public anxiety and information seeking following the H1N1 outbreak: blogs, newspaper articles, and Wikipedia visits. Health Commun. 2012;27(2):179–185. doi:10.1080/10410236.2011.571759
23. Manafo E, Wong S. Exploring older adults’ health information seeking behaviors. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2012;44(1):85–89. doi:10.1016/j.jneb.2011.05.018
24. Koffman J, Gross J, Etkind SN, Selman L. Uncertainty and COVID-19: how are we to respond? J R Soc Med. 2020;113(6):211–216. doi:10.1177/0141076820930665
25. Yu SC, Chen HR, Liu AC, Lee HY. Toward COVID-19 information: infodemic or fear of missing out? Healthcare. 2020;8(4):550. doi:10.3390/healthcare8040550
26. Liu ZW. Research progress and model construction for online health information seeking behavior. Front Res Metr Anal. 2022;6:706164. doi:10.3389/frma.2021.706164
27. Zimmerman MS, Shaw G. Health information seeking behaviour: a concept analysis. Health Inf Libr J. 2020;37(3):173–191. doi:10.1111/hir.12287
28. Slovic P. Perception of Risk. Science. 1987;236(4799):280–285. doi:10.1126/science.3563507
29. Wang G, Zhang Y, Xie S, et al. Psychological typhoon eye effect during the COVID-19 outbreak. Front Public Health. 2020;8:550051. doi:10.3389/fpubh.2020.550051
30. Zhang Y, Cao X, Wang P, et al. Emotional “inflection point” in public health emergencies with the 2019 new coronavirus pneumonia (NCP) in China. J Affect Disord. 2020;276:797–803. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2020.07.097
31. Huang L, Lei W, Xu F, Liu H, Yu L. Emotional responses and coping strategies in nurses and nursing students during Covid-19 outbreak: a comparative study. PLoS One. 2020;15(8):e0237303. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0237303
32. Zhang L, Ma M, Li D, Xin Z. The psychological typhoon eye effect during the COVID-19 outbreak in China: the role of coping efficacy and perceived threat. Glob Health. 2020;16(1):105. doi:10.1186/s12992-020-00626-8
33. Tang PM, Zhang SX, Li CH, Wei F. Geographical identification of the vulnerable groups during
34. jin ZJ, Dong X, Hui LG, Dong GY. Risk and protective factors for COVID-19 morbidity, severity, and mortality. Clin Rev Allergy Immunol. 2022;64(1):90–107. doi:10.1007/s12016-022-08921-5
35. Liu HS, Liu YR, Shen LN. Factors influencing internet health information seeking behaviors of undergraduates during COVID-19 infection pneumonia epidemic. Chin J Med Libr Inf Sci. 2021;30(2):65–74.
36. Cui XQ, Hao YH, Tang SY, et al. Reliability and validity of a self-designed COVID-19 Risk Perception Scale – a large online empirical study. Chinese J Public Health. 2021;37(7):1086–1089.
37. Feng QY, Huang CW, Jia YP, et al. Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of fear of coronavirus disease 2019 scale. Acad J Second Mil Med Univ. 2021;42(07):778–782.
38. Byrne BM. Structural Equation Modeling with AMOS: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming, 2nd Ed. Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group; 2010:396.
39. Wen Z-L, Hou J-T, Herbert WM. Structural equation model testing: cutoff criteria for goodness of fit indices and chi-square test. Chinese J Acta Psychologica Sinica. 2004;36(2):186–194.
40. Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Lee JY, Podsakoff NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J Appl Psychol. 2003;88(5):879–903. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879
41. Guazzini A, Pesce A, Marotta L, Duradoni M. Through the second wave: analysis of the psychological and perceptive changes in the Italian population during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(3):1635. doi:10.3390/ijerph19031635
42. Foa EB, Kozak MJ. Emotional processing of fear: exposure to corrective information. Psychol Bull. 1986;99(1):20–35. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.99.1.20
43. Mohammadian Khonsari N, Shafiee G, Zandifar A, et al. Comparison of psychological symptoms between infected and non-infected COVID-19 health care workers. BMC Psychiatry. 2021;21(1):170. doi:10.1186/s12888-021-03173-7
44. Jungmann SM, Witthöft M. Health anxiety, cyberchondria, and coping in the current COVID-19 pandemic: which factors are related to coronavirus anxiety? J Anxiety Disord. 2020;73:102239. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102239
45. Seyed Hashemi SG, Hosseinnezhad S, Dini S, Griffiths MD, Lin CY, Pakpour AH. The mediating effect of the cyberchondria and anxiety sensitivity in the association between problematic internet use, metacognition beliefs, and fear of COVID-19 among Iranian online population. Heliyon. 2020;6(10):e05135. doi:10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e05135
46. Chisty MA, Islam MA, Munia AT, Rahman MM, Rahman NN, Mohima M. Risk perception and information-seeking behavior during emergency: an exploratory study on COVID-19 pandemic in Bangladesh. Int J Disaster Risk Reduct. 2021;65:102580. doi:10.1016/j.ijdrr.2021.102580
47. Meltzer GY, Chang VW, Lieff SA, Grivel MM, Yang LH, Des Jarlais DC. Behavioral correlates of COVID-19 worry: stigma, knowledge, and news source. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(21):11436. doi:10.3390/ijerph182111436
48. Latif R, Rafique N, Al Asoom L, et al. Factors affecting health care professionals’ adherence to COVID-19 precautionary measures. J Multidiscip Healthc. 2022;15:2121–2128. doi:10.2147/JMDH.S384330
49. Zhang LH, Shi K, Chen XF, et al. Risk PERCEPTION CHAracteristics of Beijing Citizens on the SARS Epidemic. Chinese J Population Res. 2003;27(4):42.
50. Zhang SX, Wang Y, Jahanshahi AA, Li J, Schmitt VGH. Early evidence and predictors of mental distress of adults one month in the COVID-19 epidemic in Brazil. J Psychosom Res. 2021;142:110366. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2021.110366
51. Lateef T, Chen J, Tahir M, et al. Typhoon eye effect versus ripple effect: the role of family size on mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic in Pakistan. Glob Health. 2021;17(1):32. doi:10.1186/s12992-021-00685-5
52. Xu MX, Zheng R, Rao LL, et al. Proposals for coping with “Psychological Typhoon Eye” effect detected in COVID-19. Bulletin Chinese Acad Sci. 2020;35(3):273–282.
53. Bishara AJ, Pleskac TJ, Fridberg DJ, et al. Similar processes despite divergent behavior in two commonly used measures of risky decision making. J Behav Decis Mak. 2009;22(4):435–454. doi:10.1002/bdm.641
54. Camilleri AR, Newell BR. When and why rare events are underweighted: a direct comparison of the sampling, partial feedback, full feedback and description choice paradigms. Psychon Bull Rev. 2011;18(2):377–384. doi:10.3758/s13423-010-0040-2
55. Hertwig R, Pleskac TJ. Decisions from experience: why small samples? Cognition. 2010;115(2):225–237. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.12.009
56. Hertwig R, Erev I. The description–experience gap in risky choice. Trends Cognit Sci. 2009;13(12):517–523. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.09.004
57. Cao W, Yang Q, Zhang X. Understanding information processing and protective behaviors during the pandemic: a three-wave longitudinal study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2023;20(5):4041. doi:10.3390/ijerph20054041
58. Li CX, Zhang ZS, Li GZ. A new idea for solving multicollinearity: path analysis. Chinese J Stat Decision. 2013;(1):28–30.
© 2024 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The
full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php
and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution
- Non Commercial (unported, 3.0) License.
By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted
without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly
attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.