Back to Journals » Nature and Science of Sleep » Volume 16

Impaired Attention in Children with Obstructive Sleep Apnea: A Preliminary Study of Behavior Combined with Neuroelectrophysiology
Authors Wu Y , Wang C, Jiang Y, Zhang Y, Zheng L, Ning XL, Xu Z
Received 26 June 2024
Accepted for publication 5 December 2024
Published 11 December 2024 Volume 2024:16 Pages 1995—2007
DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/NSS.S484458
Checked for plagiarism Yes
Review by Single anonymous peer review
Peer reviewer comments 2
Editor who approved publication: Dr Valentina Alfonsi
Yunxiao Wu,1,2 Changming Wang,3 Yingchao Jiang,4 Ya Zhang,5 Li Zheng,2 Xiao-lin Ning,1,6– 8 Zhifei Xu5
1School of Instrumentation Science and Optoelectronic Engineering, Beihang University, Beijing, 100191, People’s Republic of China; 2Department of Otolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery, Beijing Children’s Hospital, Capital Medical University, National Center for Children’s Health, Beijing, 100045, People’s Republic of China; 3Department of Neurosurgery, Xuanwu Hospital, Capital Medical University, Beijing, 100053, People’s Republic of China; 4College of Psychology and Mental Health, North China University of Science and Technology, Tangshan, 063210, People’s Republic of China; 5Clinical Department of National Clinical Research Center for Respiratory Diseases, Beijing Children’s Hospital, Capital Medical University, National Center for Children’s Health, Beijing, 100045, People’s Republic of China; 6Key Laboratory of Ultra-Weak Magnetic Field Measurement Technology, Ministry of Education, School of Instrumentation and Optoelectronic Engineering, Beihang University, Beijing, 100191, People’s Republic of China; 7Institute of Large-Scale Scientific Facility and Centre for Zero Magnetic Field Science, Beihang University, Hangzhou, 310051, People’s Republic of China; 8National Institute of Extremely-Weak Magnetic Field Infrastructure, Hangzhou, 3100051, People’s Republic of China
Correspondence: Xiao-lin Ning, Beihang University, 37 Xueyuan Road, Haidian District, Beijing, 100191, People’s Republic of China, Tel +8613691352196, Email [email protected] Zhifei Xu, Beijing Children’s Hospital, Capital Medical University, National Center for Children’s Health, 56 Nanlishi Road, Xicheng, Beijing, 100045, People’s Republic of China, Tel +8613521660077, Email [email protected]
Objective: To investigate how attention is affected in children with obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) using the attention network test (ANT) combined with event-related potential (ERP) and time-frequency analysis.
Methods: Eighty-seven children aged 6– 11 years with symptoms of snoring or mouth breathing during sleep were recruited from the Sleep Center of Beijing Children’s Hospital from May to July, 2023. All participants completed the Mini-mental State Examination and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder rating scale. We acquired 32-lead electroencephalography (EEG) data while participants performed the ANT, followed by Polysomnography.
Results: Of the 87 children, 21 had no OSA, 49 had mild OSA, and 17 had moderate to severe (MS) OSA. Each group had similar questionnaire scores, similar response time and accuracy for the different ANT conditions. There are alterations in the processing of three separate components of the attentional network in children with OSA. The amplitude of the N3 component at the FZ electrode in the MS OSA group was lower than that of the non-OSA and mild OSA groups (all P< 0.05). In the executi control network phase, the energy of alpha band was higher in the MS OSA group than in the mild OSA group (Z=− 2.624, P=0.026). The mean amplitude of the N3 component at the FZ electrode was correlated with the obstructive apnea-hypopnea index (OAHI) (r=0.232, P=0.038).
Conclusion: Attention impairment was observed as a reduced N3 in the frontal area in the MS OSA group, which was correlated with the OAHI. However, questionnaire and behavioral performance did not differ significantly between groups. These findings suggest that the N3 amplitude is a sensitive neuroelectrophysiological marker of OSA-related cognitive impairment.
Keywords: OSA, child, attention, event-related potential, time-frequency
Introduction
Obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) is a sleep disorder of increasing concern in children. It is characterized by the recurrent collapse of the upper airway during sleep, which results in intermittent hypoxemia and sleep fragmentation.1 This sleep disorder is also associated with a variety of cognitive dysfunctions, such as attention deficits, executive dysfunction, abnormal behavior, and memory impairment,1–4 which adversely affect the long-term growth and development of children. Our previous study confirmed the damage of OSA to children’s attentional networks.5 However, OSA also has a higher incidence of co-morbidity with atypical attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and ADHD itself is a possible factor in attentional functioning in OSA. The attention network is a core component of cognitive function. To date, few studies have examined this network and the underlying mechanisms in pediatric patients with OSA. The combined analysis of behavioral test and neuroelectric measures, such as event-related potential (ERP) and time-frequency (TF) analysis can facilitate our understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying specific cognitive task performance.6
Attention is a complex construct that involves multiple functionally and anatomically distinct, yet interactive, networks that enable alerting, orienting, and executive (or conflict) control functions.5,7 The alerting network, which includes the right frontal and parietal cortices and thalamus, is associated with alertness and vigilance and ensures sensitivity to incoming stimuli and response readiness. The orienting network, which includes the parietal cortex and frontal eye fields, allows the selection of information via the disengagement, shifting, and re-engagement of attention in visual space. The executive control network is also known as the conflict network owing to its involvement in self-regulation and conflict resolution of thoughts, emotions, and overt responses. Regions involved include the anterior cingulate and lateral ventral prefrontal cortices.8,9 The functioning of these three distinct networks can be examined behaviorally using the attention network test (ANT).8,10
ERPs are brain-evoked potentials that reflect the electrophysiological response of the brain to a specific event (eg, a sensory, cognitive, or motor event). By analyzing different ERP components (eg, P1, N1, P2, N2, and P3), we can understand the neural dynamics of information processing.11–13 TF analysis is used to examine neurophysiological activity by analyzing signal characteristics at different times and frequencies. The energy of signals at different times and frequencies varies depending on the number of synchronously firing neurons and the precision of synchrony and reflects the neural processes that are activated in response to specific cognitive demands.6,14 Previous ERP studies using the ANT paradigm have demonstrated that cue-stimuli-related P1 and N1 components are associated with alerting and orienting network, whereas target-stimuli-related P3 components are related to the executive control network.15,16 Therefore, in our study, we considered the P1 and P3 components as important neurophysiological markers.
The aim of the present study was to investigate how attention networks are affected in children with OSA using a children’s version of the ANT combined with ERP and TF analysis.
Participants and Methods
Participants
Children were recruited from the Sleep Centre of Beijing Children’s Hospital from May 1–July 31, 2023. The inclusion criteria were: 1) children aged 6–11 years; 2) those with symptoms of snoring or mouth breathing during sleep. Exclusion criteria were: those with 1) craniofacial abnormalities, such as Down’s syndrome or Crouzon syndrome; 2) genetic metabolic disorders; 3) intellectual disability (ie Mini-mental State Examination [MMSE] score ≤ 17); 4) a history of neurological or psychiatric illness or other pathological condition that could potentially influence the study results. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Beijing Children’s Hospital. Written informed consent and assent were obtained from the parents or children (for those aged > 8 years).
MMSE
The MMSE is a widely used tool designed to quickly assess an individual’s cognitive functioning. The test encompasses seven cognitive domains: orientation in time, orientation in space, memory registration, recall, attention, calculation, and language. We excluded children who scored ≤ 17, corrected for educational level.17
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Rating Scale
The ADHD rating scale consists of 18 items. Nine items relate to attention-deficit symptoms and nine items relate to hyperactivity and impulsivity symptoms. One point is awarded for each ‘yes’ response, with higher scores indicating more severe symptoms.18
Child Version of the ANT
We used the international version of the ANT.10 The test was administered by trained staff between 2 pm and 5 pm, before polysomnography (PSG) data acquisition. The E-Prime 2.0 software (Psychology Software Tools, Sharpsburg, PA, USA) was used to program and administer the ANT. The task included four cue types (ie, no cue, central cue, double cue, and spatial cue) and three target conditions (ie, neutral, congruent, and incongruent). Children were asked to press the left or right mouse button when the central fish was pointing left or right. The paradigm comprised 24 practice sessions and 144 formal sessions, totaling approximately 25 minutes. The ANT and parent interview administrators and the PSG scorers were blind to each other’s results. Reaction time (RT) and accuracy rate were recorded for the different cue types and target conditions. The efficiency of the three networks was determined as follows: alerting network = RTno cue − RTdouble cue, where a larger difference indicates better performance of the alerting network; orienting network = RTcentral cue − RTspatial cue, where a larger difference indicates better orienting ability; and executive control network = RTincongruent − RTcongruent, where a larger difference indicates poorer executive control network function. The experimental materials and procedure are shown in Figure 1.
![]() |
Figure 1 Materials and procedure of the ANT *: cue. (A) The sequence of events of the ANT. (B) The three target conditions. (C) The four cue types. Abbreviations: RT, reaction time; ms, millisecond. |
PSG
A standard overnight PSG was performed using the E-series system (Compumedics, Melbourne, Australia) and the Alice 5 system (Respironics, Murrysville, PA, USA). Children were monitored for at least 7 hours. PSG included electroencephalography (EEG); C3/M2, C4/M1, O1/M2, and O2/M1), electrooculography, electromyography, electrocardiography, airflow monitoring (using a nasal cannula and thermistor), and assessments of chest and abdominal respiratory movement, oxygen saturation, snoring, and body position. Sleep stages and respiratory events were scored manually according to the American Academy of Sleep Medicine manual.19 The obstructive sleep apnea-hypopnea index (OAHI) is the number of episodes per hour of obstructive apnea, mixed apnea, and obstructive hypopnea. An OAHI score of 1 < OAHI ≤ 5 events/hour (e/h), 5 < OAHI ≤ 10 e/h, and > 10 e/h were categorized as mild, moderate, and severe OSA, respectively.1
EEG Acquisition and Data Processing
EEG was recorded while participants completed the ANT using the Grael EEG system (Compumedics, Melbourne, Australia) and a 32-channel saline EEG cap. Electrode placement on the EEG cap followed the international 10–20 system. The impedance of all electrodes was kept below 30 kΩ. The EEG was digitized at 2048 hz with a 0.1–100 hz band pass, including a 60-Hz notch filter, and stored for offline averaging. The data were then analyzed offline and re-referenced to averaged whole-brain electrodes in epochs of 1000 ms, which included a 200 ms pre-stimulus period for baseline correction. Epochs exceeding an amplitude of ±100 μV, identified via visual inspection, were rejected as artifacts. The ERP components analyzed were P1 (100–200 ms), N2 (200–300 ms), and P3 (250–400 ms) of the posterior electrodes (ie, P3, P4, PZ, O1, O2, and OZ) and N1 (100–200 ms), P2 (150–275 ms), and N3 (300–500 ms) of the anterior electrodes (ie, FZ, FCZ, and CZ).
The time window used for the TF analysis was −200–1000 ms, and we selected the continuous wavelet transform analysis method. Baseline correction of the energy values was −200–−100 ms, and there was no difference in the energy values of the spectral analysis baseline after the baseline correction. Energy values were extracted for the following frequency bands: delta (1–4 hz), theta (4–7 hz), alpha (8–13 hz), beta (13–30 hz), low beta (13–16 hz), high beta (16–30 hz, and gamma (30–45 hz). These frequency bands were chosen because each captures the distinct neural oscillatory activity associated with specific cognitive processes.
Statistical Analysis
We used the SPSS 26.0 software (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) for statistical analysis. Normally distributed data are expressed as means ± standard deviations, and data with a skewed distribution are expressed as medians and interquartile ranges (P25, P75). For normally distributed data, groups were compared using an analysis of variance (ANOVA). Subsequent pairwise comparisons to explore between-group differences were conducted using Bonferroni or Tamhane’s T2 procedures according to the homogeneity of variances significance. For non-normally distributed data, we used non-parametric tests to evaluate group differences. For comparisons of numerical data, we used the chi-squared test. Correlations between EEG parameters and PSG and clinical parameters were analyzed using partial correlation to correct for age, sex, and body mass index. All tests were two-sided and P < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Results
A total of 109 children aged 6–11 years who underwent PSG at the sleep center from May 1, 2023, to July 31, 2023, were recruited. One patient had a history of epilepsy, 1 patient had a history of febrile seizures, 1 patient had a history of ADHD, 3 patients had unsuccessful data acquisition, 1 patient had a history of osteomyelitis, and 15 patients were excluded because of poor EEG data quality. This resulted in 87 children who were included in the analysis. A data collection flowchart is shown in Figure 2.
![]() |
Figure 2 Flowchart of data collection. |
Demographic Characteristics and Questionnaire Scores
The demographic characteristics and questionnaire scores of the included children are shown in Table 1. There were 21 children without OSA, 49 with mild OSA, and 17 with moderate to severe (MS) OSA. The body mass index of the MS OSA group was higher than that of the non-OSA group (P < 0.01). Age, sex, years of education, MMSE score, and ADHD scale scores did not differ significantly between the three groups.
![]() |
Table 1 Demographic Characteristics and Questionnaire Scores of All Children |
PSG results
The PSG data of the three groups of children are shown in Table 2. Results showed expected differences, which signified a higher number of respiratory events, more disturbed sleep architecture, and poorer blood oxygenation in the MS OSA group than in the non-OSA and mild OSA groups.
![]() |
Table 2 Comparison of Polysomnographic Results for All Children |
ANT Results
The ANT data of the three groups of children are presented in Table 3. There were no significant differences between the three groups for response time or accuracy in any of the conditions. There were no significant group differences in the efficiency of the three attention networks (ie, the alerting, orienting, and executive control networks).
![]() |
Table 3 Comparisons of the Attention Network Test Results for All Children |
ERP Results
There were no significant group differences in the mean amplitude or latency of the P1 component for the P3, PZ, P4, O1, OZ, and O2 electrodes in the alerting network (Supplementary Table 1). The mean amplitude and latency of the P1 component at P3, PZ, P4, O1, and OZ electrodes in the orienting network were not significantly different among the three groups. However, the latency of the P1 component at the O2 electrode was shorter in the mild OSA group than in the non-OSA group (Z=2.630, P=0.026; Figure 3 and Supplementary Table 2).
![]() |
Figure 3 Event-related potential plots of the spatial and central cue types for the three groups. Abbreviations: MS OSA, moderate to severe OSA; OSA, obstructive sleep apnea. |
For the executive control network, the mean amplitude of the N3 component at the FZ electrode in the MS OSA group was lower than that of the non-OSA and mild OSA groups (MS OSA group vs non-OSA group, Z=−2.905, P=0.011; MS OSA group vs mild OSA group, Z=−2.538, P=0.033; Figure 4 and Supplementary Table 3). There were no significant differences among the three groups for the other conditions.
![]() |
Figure 4 Event-related potential plots of the incongruent and congruent target conditions for the three groups. Abbreviations: MS, moderate to severe; OSA, obstructive sleep apnea. |
TF Analysis Results
For the alerting network phase, the energy of the high-beta band was higher in the mild OSA group than in the non-OSA group at the PZ electrode (Z=−2.766, P=0.017) and in the MS OSA group at the O2 electrode (Z=3.164, P=0.005). There were no significant differences among the three groups for the other conditions (Figure 5A and B and Supplementary Table 4).
There were no significant differences among groups for the energy values in the orienting network phase (Supplementary Table 5).
For the executive control network phase, the energy of the alpha band was higher in the MS OSA group than in the mild OSA group at the PZ electrode (Z=−2.624, P=0.026). There were no other significant differences (Figure 6 and Supplementary Table 6).
Correlation Analysis
The partial correlation results are shown in Table 4. The mean amplitude of the N3 component at the FZ electrode was significantly correlated with the OAHI (r=0.232, P=0.038). The energy of the high-beta band at the O2 electrode in the alerting network phase was correlated with N2%.
![]() |
Table 4 Correlations Between Electrophysiological Parameters and Polysomnographic and Clinical Indicators |
Discussion
We investigated the function of the attention networks in children without OSA and those with mild and MS OSA. Questionnaire and behavioral results were similar across the three groups. However, the neuroelctrophysiological indicators of the OSA groups were significantly different from those of the non-OSA group. Specifically, children with mild OSA were hyper-vigilant, which caused their orienting network to be activated earlier to ensure normality of more advanced cognitive processing. Children with MS OSA had a sleepier brain when performing an executive control task; their cognitive control was impaired, as reflected by their significantly reduced N3 amplitude at the FZ electrode. In addition, the N3 amplitude at the FZ electrode correlated with the OAHI, which is an indicator of OSA severity. Our findings suggest that neuroelctrophysiological indicators can help detect OSA-related neurocognitive impairment earlier and more accurately than questionnaires and behavioral tests.
Before proceeding to the discussion of medically relevant content, there is a technical issue that needs to be clarified. The ERP waveform in the posterior area is opposite to that in the anterior area because of the averaging of electrodes across the whole brain, which were used as reference electrodes. Therefore, the N3 component of the frontal region electrodes in our study is comparable to the P3 component reported in previous literature.20
Alerting and Orienting Networks
Alerting is defined as heightened response preparation in response to external warning stimuli. Orienting refers to the ability to prioritize sensory input by selecting a modality or location and shifting attention (eg, by distracting and refocusing attention).8 The P1 component, an early component of the attention process, is enhanced with increasing attention load, which reflects the early processing of stimuli.16,21 In addition, the attended stimulus can evoke a larger N1 amplitude.21 The amplitudes of the P1 and N1 components reflect a sensory gain control mechanism that leads to perceptual processing of the attended stimulus.22 ERP studies have shown that P1 and N1 components appear in parietal and occipital regions 80–200 ms after stimulus presentation and are related to alerting and orienting functions of the attention network.22 Our study showed that children with mild OSA had higher energy in the high-beta band and shorter latency of the P1 component than those with non-OSA. We speculate that when children with mild OSA perform the ANT task, they are hyper-vigilant approximately 100–200 ms after presentation of the cue stimuli, which results in early activation of the orienting network. Hyper-vigilance may be a compensatory mechanism for OSA-related sleep disturbances that enables children to remain alert during the daytime to cope with the demands of learning and daily activities. Such a compensatory mechanism has been reported in other diseases. One study using resting-state functional MRI showed increased functional connectivity from the thalamus to cuneus, fusiform gyrus, and lingual gyrus in isolated rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder patients compared with healthy controls, which were thought to reflect a compensatory mechanism for cognitive impairment.23 We similarly suggest that the mild OSA group possessed such a compensatory mechanism because they exhibited comparable or higher efficiency of the alerting network during the ANT, which may indicate the early stages of OSA-related cognition impairment.
Executive Control Network
The executive control network involves conflict control and resolution during habitual response inhibition, decision-making, and error detection.8 The P3 component is a latent component commonly used in studies of executive control,16 and its amplitude and latency reflect the amount of attention resources allocated to external events in the environment and cognitive processing speed, respectively.24,25 A smaller P3 component indicates a reduction in conflict-resolving ability, where the smaller the amplitude, the fewer attention resources available.16 Reductions in the P3 component have been reported in patients with ADHD, which suggests that these patients recruit fewer cortical resources during attentionally demanding tasks.6 In our study, children with MS OSA showed a significantly smaller N3 component than non-OSA and mild OSA children, which indicated that their attentional resources were limited and that they were at risk of resource depletion in the later stages of the task. In addition, we observed that children with MS OSA exhibited higher levels of sleepiness, as indicated by the higher energy of the alpha band in the executive control network phase. The posterior-occipital alpha band is considered an index of cognitive control and an inverse measure of cortical excitability.21 Taken together, we speculate that children with MS OSA are in a state of low arousal and are unable to adequately allocate attentional resources when performing an attentionally demanding task. This is consistent with a previous report that demonstrated that intermittent hypoxemia and sleep fragmentation in children with OSA results in daytime tiredness and sleepiness; moreover, this sleepiness is particularly pronounced during cognitively demanding tasks.26 Furthermore, such cognitive deficits in children with OSA may have long-term effects on academic performance and daily functioning. Therefore, early diagnosis and effective treatment of OSA are crucial for improving the cognitive development of children with OSA.
Neuroelectrophysiological Indicators Have the Potential to Be More Sensitive Than Questionnaire and Behavior Measures
There were no significant differences in the MMSE or ADHD scale scores among the groups. This may have been because the MMSE and ADHD scale are not sufficiently sensitive in detecting cognitive deficits associated with OSA. We found similar results for the ANT behavioral test. However, the neuroelectrophysiological indices were significantly different among groups, and were correlated with the OAHI score. Furthermore, our findings are consistent with our previous large-scale behavioral observations, which showed that children with mild OSA have the most hyperactive alerting network among healthy controls and children with MS OSA.5 By taking advantage of the high temporal resolution of ERPs, we revealed that mild OSA primarily impacts the early stages of attentional network processes. In this group, this change is still in a compensatory phase; thus, their executive control functions normally. However, MS OSA results in poor executive control, which likely signifies a stage of decompensation of the neurocognitive processes associated with OSA. A longitudinal follow-up study is needed to clarify this hypothesis.
Limitation
This is the first study to evaluate the attention networks of children with varying severity levels of OSA while measuring EEG during the attention task. However, there are several limitations that must be considered in the interpretation of our results. Our study was conducted in a relatively small number of participants and lacked a healthy control group. In addition, because it was a cross-sectional study, the compensatory phase in children with mild OSA and progression from the compensatory phase in MS OSA could not be established. Furthermore, whether the neuroelectrophysiological indicator alterations can be restored by OSA intervention needs to be explored. In addition, it should be noted that a number of factors can influence attention levels, including the educational attainment of the child’s parents, the presence of ADHD and the emotional state of the child at the time. Despite our efforts to control for these variables, it is important to acknowledge that they may still have an impact on the results.
Conclusion
This study provided electrophysiological evidence of alterations in the processing of three separate components of the attentional network in children with OSA. The alerting network is hyperactive in children with mild OSA and that the attention and cognition are maintained via a compensatory mechanism. Children with MS OSA showed higher levels of sleepiness during a control task and had difficulty integrating and evaluating stimulus information, which indicated impaired attentional resource allocation and inhibitory control. The neuroelectrophysiological indicators have the potential to be more sensitive than questionnaire and behavior measures to detect OSA-related cognitive impairment.
Declaration of Helsinki
This study complies with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Abbreviations
ADHD, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; ANT, attention network test; EEG, electroencephalography; ERP, event-related potential; MMSE, mini-mental State Examination, OAHI, obstructive sleep apnea-hypopnea index; OSA, obstructive sleep apnea; TF, time-frequency.
Acknowledgments
Many thanks to all the technicians in sleep center for screening the PSG testing results. Thanks to all children and their parents for their active participation.
An unauthorized version of the Chinese MMSE was used by the study team without permission, however this has now been rectified with PAR.
The MMSE is a copyrighted instrument and may not be used or reproduced in whole or in part, in any form or language, or by any means without written permission of PAR (www.parinc.com).
Funding
This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (82070092).
Disclosure
The author reports no conflicts of interest in this work.
References
1. Working Group of Chinese Guideline for the Diagnosis and Treatment of Childhood OSA, Subspecialty Group of Pediatrics, Society of Otorhinolaryngology Head and Neck Surgery, Chinese Medical Association, Subspecialty Group of Respiratory Diseases, Society of Pediatrics, Chinese Medical Association, et al. Chinese guideline for the diagnosis and treatment of childhood obstructive sleep apnea (2020)[J]. Pediatr Investig. 2021;5(3):167–187. doi:10.1002/ped4.12284
2. Tang Y, Yang C, Wang C, Wu Y, Xu Z, Ni X. Impaired declarative memory consolidation in children with REM sleep-related obstructive sleep apnea. J Clin Sleep Med. 2024;20(3):417–425. doi:10.5664/jcsm.10892
3. Marcus CL, Brooks LJ, Draper KA, et al. Diagnosis and management of childhood obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Pediatrics. 2012;130(3):576–584. doi:10.1542/peds.2012-1671
4. Blechner M, Williamson AA. Consequences of Obstructive Sleep Apnea in Children. Curr Prob Pediatr Ad. 2016;46(1):19–26.
5. Wu Y, Wang Y, Wang C, et al. Characteristics of the attentional network in children with sleep-disordered breathing. Nat Sci Sleep. 2023;15:719–727. doi:10.2147/NSS.S413330
6. Hasler R, Perroud N, Meziane HB, et al. Attention-related EEG markers in adult ADHD. Neuropsychologia. 2016;87:120–133. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.05.008
7. Posner MI, Petersen SE. The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1990;13:25–42. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325
8. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI. Testing the efficiency and Independence of attentional networks. J Cognitive Neurosci. 2002;14(3):340–347. doi:10.1162/089892902317361886
9. Posner MI, Rothbart MK. Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:1–23. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085516
10. Rueda MR, Fan J, McCandliss BD, et al. Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42(8):1029–1040. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.012
11. Picton TW, Bentin S, Berg P, et al. Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteria. Psychophysiology. 2000;37(2):127–152. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3720127
12. Zhang M, Gong X, Jia J, Wang X. Unpleasant odors affect alerting attention in young men: an event-related potential study using the attention network test. Front Neurosci-Switz. 2021;15:781997. doi:10.3389/fnins.2021.781997
13. Helfrich RF, Knight RT. Cognitive neurophysiology: event-related potentials. handb clin neurol. 2019;160:543–558.
14. Deiber MP, Ibanez V, Missonnier P, Rodriguez C, Giannakopoulos P. Age-associated modulations of cerebral oscillatory patterns related to attention control. Neuroimage. 2013;82:531–546. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.037
15. Neuhaus AH, Urbanek C, Opgen-Rhein C, et al. Event-related potentials associated with attention network test. Int J Psychophysiol. 2010;76(2):72–79. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.02.005
16. An X, Tao G, Zhang X, Ma H, Wang Y. Attention network changes of high-altitude migrants. Aerosp Med Hum Perf. 2022;93(11):791–799. doi:10.3357/AMHP.6061.2022
17. Kang JM, Cho YS, Park S, et al. Montreal cognitive assessment reflects cognitive reserve. Bmc Geriatr. 2018;18(1):261. doi:10.1186/s12877-018-0951-8
18. Consensus on pediatric clinical practice of early identification, standardized diagnosis and treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Zhonghua Er Ke Za Zhi. 2020;58(3):188–193. doi:10.3760/cma.j.issn.0578-1310.2020.03.006
19. Berry RB, Quan SF, Abreu AR, et al. The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specifications. Version 2.6. Darien, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2020.
20. Luck SJ, Kappenman ES. The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components. Oxford University Press, Oxford;2011
21. Ahumada-Mendez F, Lucero B, Avenanti A, et al. Affective modulation of cognitive control: a systematic review of EEG studies. Physiol Behav. 2022;249:113743. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2022.113743
22. Zhang D, Zhang X, Ma H, Wang Y, Ma H, Liu M. Competition among the attentional networks due to resource reduction in Tibetan indigenous residents: evidence from event-related potentials. Sci Rep-UK. 2018;8(1):610. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-18886-7
23. Byun JI, Kim HW, Kang H, et al. Altered resting-state thalamo-occipital functional connectivity is associated with cognition in isolated rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder. Sleep Med. 2020;69:198–203. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2020.01.010
24. Hillman CH, Kamijo K, Scudder M. A review of chronic and acute physical activity participation on neuroelectric measures of brain health and cognition during childhood. Prev Med. 2011;52 Suppl 1:S21–8. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2011.01.024
25. Abramov DM, Pontes M, Pontes AT, et al. Visuospatial information processing load and the ratio between parietal cue and target P3 amplitudes in the attentional network test. Neurosci Lett. 2017;647:91–96. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2017.03.031
26. Beebe DW, Gozal D. Obstructive sleep apnea and the prefrontal cortex: towards a comprehensive model linking nocturnal upper airway obstruction to daytime cognitive and behavioral deficits. J Sleep Res. 2002;11(1):1–16. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2869.2002.00289.x
© 2024 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The
full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php
and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution
- Non Commercial (unported, 3.0) License.
By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted
without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly
attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.