Back to Journals » Psychology Research and Behavior Management » Volume 17

The Combined Impact of Physical Activity and Sedentary Behavior on Executive Functions in Older Adults: A Cross-Sectional Study

Authors Chen HY, Hung CS, Wu TT , Ren FF, Chang YK, Chen FT 

Received 2 August 2024

Accepted for publication 30 October 2024

Published 7 November 2024 Volume 2024:17 Pages 3851—3861

DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S486089

Checked for plagiarism Yes

Review by Single anonymous peer review

Peer reviewer comments 2

Editor who approved publication: Professor Mei-Chun Cheung



Hung-Yu Chen,1 Chen-Sin Hung,2 Ting-Ting Wu,3 Fei-Fei Ren,4 Yu-Kai Chang,2,5,6 Feng-Tzu Chen3

1Department of Sports Medicine, China Medical University, Taichung, Taiwan; 2Department of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, National Taiwan Normal University, Taipei, Taiwan; 3Department of Kinesiology, National Tsing Hua University, Hsinchu, Taiwan; 4Department of Physical Education, Beijing Language and Culture University, Beijing, People’s Republic of China; 5Social Emotional Education and Development Center, National Taiwan Normal University, Taipei, Taiwan; 6Institute of Sport Science and Innovations, Lithuanian Sports University, Kaunas, Lithuania

Correspondence: Feng-Tzu Chen, Department of Kinesiology, National Tsing Hua University, No. 521, Nanda Road, East District, Hsinchu City, 300, Taiwan, Email [email protected]

Background: The interplay between physical activity (PA) and sedentary behavior (SB) significantly influences cognitive health in older adults, with executive functions (EFs) being particularly vulnerable to lifestyle factors. However, previous research on older adults focused mainly on PA and is limited due to the lack of comprehensive consideration of other factors that influence EFs. Current guidelines suggest an association between sedentary behavior (SB) and EFs, yet few studies have examined the combined effects of PA and SB on EFs.
Objective: This cross-sectional study aimed to explore the relationship between PA, SB, and EFs in older adults.
Methods: A total of 116 healthy older adults aged ≥ 65 years were recruited and categorized into four groups based on activity lifestyles: higher PA and higher SB (PHSH); higher PA and lower SB (PHSL); lower PA and higher SB (PLSH); and lower PA and lower SB (PLSL). EFs were assessed using the Tower of London (TOL) task and the Stroop test, which measure high-order EFs (planning and problem-solving) and core EFs (inhibitory control), respectively.
Results: The PHSL group performed better on the TOL task, with lower total move scores and shorter total problem-solving times, compared to the PLSH group. The total move score of the PLSL group was better, and the total problem-solving time of the PHSH group was shorter than those of the PLSH group. However, the Stroop test scores did not differ between the four groups.
Conclusion: These findings suggest that increasing PA or reducing SB selectively enhances executive functions, particularly in planning and problem-solving, while less impacting inhibitory control in older adults. This highlights the importance of considering the nuanced effects of PA and SB on different aspects of executing functioning in older adults.

Keywords: cognition, planning, inhibition, cognitive decline, sitting

Introduction

Cognitive function (CF) decline, considered a normal part of aging, increases at a rate of 0.04 to 0.05 standard deviation units per year.1,2 CF decline, ranging from mild cognitive impairment (MCI) to severe cognitive diseases such as dementia, has become a significant societal concern. For instance, a new case of dementia is estimated to be detected every four seconds globally.3 In 2010, approximately 30.1 million people were diagnosed with dementia worldwide, with this number projected to rise by 30%, reaching 65.7 million by 2030 and continuing to increase to 106.2 million by 2050.4,5 Effective treatments for CF decline have yet to be developed; therefore, it is anticipated that the global economic burden associated with healthcare would increase.6,7

Physical activity (PA) is a promising strategy for preventing CF declines.8,9 Cohort studies have shown that PA reduces CF decline and helps maintain high-order CFs such as executive function (EF).10–12 EF, also known as executive control, encompasses the CFs necessary to regulate purposeful, goal-directed behaviors, including high-order EFs, eg, planning and problem-solving, and core EFs, eg, inhibitory control, working memory, and switching.13–15 High-order and core EFs are crucial for adapting to new situations, managing tasks efficiently, and achieving long-term goals, making them essential for daily functioning and behavior regulation.16,17 Importantly, current systematic reviews found that PA positively impacts several facets of EF in adults over 50,18,19 suggesting that PA benefits multiple EFs in the older population.

Increased PA is associated with improved core EFs but has limited effects on high-order EFs. Previous studies showed that adults who engage in PA perform better in inhibitory control,20 working memory,21 and switching.22 However, the effects of PA on EFs related to planning and problem-solving remain underexplored. The current meta-analysis showed that older adults undergoing PA interventions in randomized controlled trials improved overall EFs (the effect size was 0.21) and the three core EFs (the effect size ranged from 0.14 to 0.27).23 However, due to limited data on high-order EFs, their effect size could not be evaluated in the review. Indeed, several reviews supported the findings and further suggested that future research should focus on multiple facets of executing functioning in older adults.24,25

The optimal duration of PA for improving or maintaining EF in older adults is still unclear. Most studies on this topic focused on basic CF and yielded conflicting results. For instance, a cross-sectional study observed an inverse-U relationship between PA duration and CF, with moderate-intensity PA for 22 to 38 minutes per day (154 to 266 minutes per week) providing the most benefits.26 Conversely, the current study found that longer durations of moderate to vigorous-intensity PA, exceeding current PA recommendations, were associated with decreased CF decline.27 To clarify this inconsistency, future studies should explore the relationship between PA and EFs in the older population to determine the optimal benefits for EFs.

Evidence suggests that higher levels of sedentary behavior (SB), which is defined as remaining in a sitting or reclining posture with a low energy expenditure (<1.5 metabolic equivalents [METs]), increase morbidity and mortality risk.28 Furthermore, SB also increases the risk of dementia and is associated with deficiencies in EFs across the lifespan.29–31 Specifically, longer SB times negatively influence EF, even if recommended PA levels, are achieved.32 However, current evidence suggests no association between SB time and EF in older adults,33 which suggests that the relationship between specific SB time and EF remains to be further investigated.

Although PA and SB times are critical factors influencing EFs, their consequences show opposing trends. Specifically, PA positively influences EF through increased energy expenditure and muscle contraction,34,35 whereas SB is associated with poor EF due to prolonged sitting and limited movement.36,37 Individuals who engage in sufficient PA but prolonged SB can be described as being both physically active and sedentary. Therefore, PA and SB patterns are independent, with four activity lifestyles, including higher PA and higher SB time (PHSH); higher PA and lower SB time (PHSL); lower PA and higher SB time (PLSH); and lower PA and lower SB time (PLSL). Only two studies have examined the combined effects of PA and SB on EF. Specifically, one study found EF deficits in children with high PA and SB time compared to those with low PA and SB time.38 Another study of Brazilian adults39 (median age = 30 years) reported that higher levels of moderate to vigorous intensity PA were associated with better EF performance. At the same time, less than 9 hours of SB daily enhanced EF. In summary, more PA and less SB appear beneficial for EFs. While a few studies have considered both PA and SB in relation to EFs in children and adults, none have focused on older adults or examined both high-level and core EFs in a single study.

This study aimed to investigate the combined effects of PA and SB on multiple EFs in a community sample of older adults. It examined four activity lifestyles—PHSL, PHSH, PLSL, and PLSH—to understand their impacts on core and high-level EFs. Given the evidence linking PA to EFs, we hypothesized that the EF performance of older adults with a lifestyle of higher PA, ie, PHSL and PHSH, would be better than that of older adults with unfit lifestyles of lower PA and higher SB, ie, PLSH. In addition, we hypothesized that the EF performance of individuals with shorter SB time, ie, PHSL and PLSL, would be better than that of individuals with unfit lifestyles of lower PA and higher SB, ie, PLSH.

Methods

Participants

Volunteers aged ≥65 years were recruited using recruitment flyers distributed in local gyms and senior universities in Taichung, Taiwan. All participants were contacted by telephone and asked to complete a Health Screening Questionnaire. Their responses were compared with predetermined inclusion criteria to assess their suitability for fitness testing. Participant inclusion criteria were an age of 65 years or more, no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, living independently in the community, normal or corrected-to-normal vision (20/40), a score of 27 or more on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), and no sensory and language deficits.

The International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) and the Sedentary Behavior Questionnaire (SBQ) were used to assess participants’ PA and SB. The IPAQ, a validated tool for quantifying PA, measures the frequency (days per week) and duration (minutes per day) of activities at varying intensities, including vigorous, moderate, and light activities, with results expressed in Metabolic Equivalent Task minutes per week (MET-min/week) for classification into activity levels.40 The SBQ, a complementary self-report tool, captures time spent in sedentary activities, such as watching television, using a computer, reading, and driving. These questionnaires comprehensively overview participants’ PA and SB patterns.

Eligible participants were allocated into four groups, PHSL, PHSH, PLSL, and PLSH, based on PA expenditure and SB time: (1) higher PA (>1200 METs/week); (2) lower PA (≤1200 METs/week); (3) higher SB time (>8 hours/day or >3360 minutes/week); and (4) lower SB time (≤8 hours/day or ≤3360 minutes/week) over the previous three months.41 All participants had to visit the laboratory twice, and each evaluation was performed individually (lasting about 1 hour). During the first visit, they completed a self-questionnaire (providing demographic information, IPAQ, and SBQ) and performed the two-minute step test (2MST) to verify their physical fitness. All participants completed two EF tasks, the Tower of London (TOL) task and the Stroop test, during the second visit. Before participation, written informed consent was obtained according to the study protocol approved by the Institutional Review Board of China Medical University (No. CMUH111-REC3-097).

The Two-Minute Step Test

The 2MST is part of the senior fitness test.42 Participants were asked to march in place, lifting their knees above a set height, and the number of steps taken within two minutes was recorded. This test is advantageous because it takes only a limited amount of space and a short time and no expensive equipment is required.43 Previous studies have shown that the 2MST is correlated with the six-minute walking test44 and is a reliable measure of PA levels in healthy individuals.45

Executive Function Assessments

The Tower of London-Drexel University (TOLDX) task was used to assess high-order EFs, especially the planning and problem-solving aspect.46 The task involves two identical wooden boards (30 × 7 × 10 cm) and two sets of three colored beads (blue, green, and red). Each board has three vertical pegs: the tallest peg holds three beads, the middle peg holds two, and the shortest peg holds one. Participants were presented with ten problems (ranging from 3 to 7 moves) and asked to use the fewest moves to achieve the goal position from the start position (Figure 1). Five indices were computed:47 (1) total move score (TMS), ie, the number of actual moves minus the minimum number of moves; (2) total correct score (TCS), ie, the number of problems solved in which only the minimum number of moves was used; (3) total initial time (TIT), ie, the time when the participant initially moved the first bead; (4) total executive time (TET), ie, the time from initiation of the first move to the solving of the problem; and (5) total planning-solving time (TPST), ie, the sum of initial time and executive time. The TOL task required 20–25 minutes.

Figure 1 The example of the start and goal position of the Tower of London Task.

The Stroop test in this study mainly assessed the core EF, ie, inhibitory control, and information processing speed.48 Participants were instructed to verbally state the color of the stimulus as quickly as possible. The modified Stroop test included five conditions: word, congruent, color, neutral, and incongruent,49 and each condition included 50 stimuli (Figure 2). Specifically, the incongruent condition was regarded as examining inhibitory control. In contrast, other conditions are assessed to determine the information processing speed. The word condition involved color names presented in black ink, eg, “RED” in black. The congruent condition involved color names in ink of the matching color, eg, “RED” in red. The color condition consisted of colored rectangles. The neutral condition had unrelated words in colored ink, eg, “PAPER” in red. The incongruent condition had color names in different-colored ink, eg, “RED” in blue. Stimuli were displayed on a sheet, and participants named the colored stimuli from top to bottom and left to right. Each participant practiced with five stimuli before starting each condition. Reaction time was recorded using a handheld stopwatch. The Stroop test took about 10–15 minutes.

Figure 2 The example of the incongruent condition of the Stroop test.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 22.0, with a significance set at p <0.05. The participants’ background data and intelligent indices, ie, age, height, body mass index, MMSE, digit forward span, digit backward span, IPAQ, sitting time, and 2MST, were analyzed using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) to compare differences between the four groups (PHSL, PHSH, PLSL, and PLSH). One-way ANOVA was also used to determine differences in TOL performance (TMS, TCS, TIT, TET, and TPST) between the groups. Furthermore, a two-way (group × task condition) ANOVA was used to compare differences in reaction time between Stroop test task conditions (word, congruent, color, neutral, and incongruent) and between the four groups. The Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied when sphericity was violated, and post-hoc analyses were performed using Bonferroni-adjusted multiple comparisons.

Results

Participants

The sample comprised 116 participants distributed across the four groups (Figure 3): PHSL (n = 35), PHSH (n = 23), PLSL (n = 33), and PLSH (n = 25). A power analysis was conducted, considering the effect sizes observed in related studies.50,51 The analysis determined that a sample size of 100–120 participants would provide sufficient power (80%) to detect meaningful differences between groups, ensuring the study is adequately powered to identify significant effects. One-way ANOVA revealed no significant differences between the groups in participants’ backgrounds, ie, age, height, and BMI, and intelligence indices, ie, MMSE, digit forward span, and digit backward span (p > 0.05). However, significant differences were found in fitness indices, including IPAQ, sitting time, and two-minute step (all p = 0.000), confirming the appropriate assignment of participants to the lifestyle groups. Post-hoc analysis showed the following. The IPAQ scores of the participants in the PHSH and PHSL groups were higher than those in the PLSH and PLSL groups (p < 0.05), but no significant difference was observed between PHSH and PHSL (p > 0.05). Participants in the PHSL and PLSL groups had less SB time than PHSH and PLSH participants, with no significant difference between PHSH and PHSL or between PHSL and PLSL (p > 0.05). The step count of participants in the PHSH group was higher than that of the other three groups (p < 0.05), with no significant difference observed between these three groups (p > 0.05). The means and SDs of participants’ demographic data are summarized for the four groups in Table 1.

Table 1 Participants’ Demographic Data Among Four Groups

Figure 3 The distribution of recruited participants based on their physical activity expenditure and sedentary behavior time.

Abbreviations: PHSL, higher physical activity and lower sitting time; PHSH, higher physical activity and higher sitting time; PLSL, lower physical activity and lower sitting time; PLSH, lower physical activity and higher sitting time.

The Tower of London Task

The results of the TOL task are presented in Table 2. One-way ANOVA revealed a significant difference in the TMS between groups (F3,112 = 3.449, p = 0.019). Post-hoc analysis showed that the TMS of the PHSL group was significantly lower than that of the PLSH group (p = 0.010). The TMS of the PLSL group was significantly lower than that of the PLSH group (p = 0.005). One-way ANOVA revealed a significant difference in the TPST (F3,112 = 2.771, p = 0.045). Post-hoc analysis showed that the TPST was shorter in the PHSL group than that in the PLSH group (p = 0.006); it was also shorter in the PHSH group than in the PLSH group (p = 0.038). However, TCS (p = 0.325), TIT (p = 0.385), and TET (p = 0.071) did not differ significantly between the four groups (Figure 4).

Table 2 Performance of the Tower of London Task Among Four Groups

Figure 4 The performance of the Tower of London task among four groups.

Note: * represents a significant difference (p < 0.05).

Stroop Test

The results for the Stroop test are presented in Table 3. Two-way ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of task condition (F4,112 = 363.081, p < 0.001). Post-hoc analysis showed that the reaction time was longest (83.36 ± 2.87 ms) for the incongruent condition, followed by the neutral condition (49.69 ± 1.21 ms), square condition (38.09 ± 1.19 ms), word condition (27.61 ± 0.79 ms), and congruent condition (26.25 ± 0.74 ms). However, group (F3,112 = 0.454, p = 0.715) and the group × task condition interaction (F12,112 = 1.408, p = 0.158) did not have a significant effect on reaction time.

Table 3 Performance of the Stroop Test Among Four Groups

Discussion

The present study examined the combined effects of PA and SB on high-order and core EFs in older adults. In the case of high-order EFs, specifically planning and problem-solving, the TOL task found that the TMS of participants in the PHSL and PLSL groups was significantly lower than that of the PLSH group. Furthermore, participants in the PHSL and PHSH groups exhibited a shorter TPST than those in the PLSH group. However, in the case of the core EF, inhibitory control, the differences in reaction time between the task conditions in the Stroop test did not depend on the lifestyle group, ie, the interaction between the group and the task condition was not significant. These results suggest that EF differences are selectively sensitive, with high-order EFs, rather than the core EF, being primarily affected, underscoring the importance of considering the nuanced effects of PA and SB on different aspects of executive functioning in older adults.

Based on the novelty of the TOL task in assessing the effects of PA and SB time on high-order EFs, our study cross-sectionally investigated EF benefits related to planning and problem-solving across different activity lifestyles. We found that older adults with high-fitness lifestyles, ie, the PHSL group, had a lower TMS and shorter TPST than those with a low-fitness lifestyle, ie, the PLSH group. According to previous studies,47,52 the TMS primarily measures the quality of planning and problem-solving efficiency, and TPST represents the overall speed of planning and planning efficiency. Therefore, older individuals achieving a higher TMS and TPST during the TOL task transformed current mental representations into goal states by considering multiple suitable strategies.46,52 Interestingly, we also observed that the PLSL group had a lower TMS, and the PHSH group had a shorter TPST than the PLSH group. Therefore, we concluded that participants with lower SB time, regardless of PA expenditure, ie, the PHSL and PLSL groups, showed better move-related performance in planning quality in the TOL task. In contrast, participants with higher PA, regardless of SB time, ie, the PHSL and PHSH groups, showed better speed-related performance in planning efficiency.

While current guidelines recommend a balance between PA and SB for promoting health, SB time consistently exceeds the time spent on PA during waking hours.53 It is worth noting that in our study the TMS of older adults who engaged less in SB, even with lower PA, ie, the PLSL group, was lower than that of the individuals with lower PA and higher SB, ie, the PLSH group, implying that reducing SB time had a positive impact on EFs in older adults. This finding is supported by a previous study,26 which showed a significant relationship between SB and global CF. Similarly, systematic reviews have suggested that lower SB time is associated with greater CF in late-middle-aged adults54 and older adults.55 Our findings further broaden the association between SB and EFs and suggest that older adults with lower SB time are more proficient than those with a low-fitness lifestyle in performing the TOL task, which requires the ability to recognize initial and goal states, anticipate future events, and store representations of intermediate states that guide movements from the initial to the goal state (Culbertson et al, 2004; Culbertson & Zillmer, 1998).

The combined benefits of increasing PA or decreasing SB time in high-order EFs, ie, the TOL task, in our study, may be explained by the release of molecules important for neuronal health, for example, during increased cerebral blood flow and cerebrovascular reactivity in the frontal lobe, which is strongly associated with executive functioning.56,57 Furthermore, a recent review demonstrated that higher PA positively impacts brain structure and function in the older population.58 Similarly, lower SB time can mitigate the associated risks of cognitive function59 and positively influence brain health in older adults.60 Considering these potential mechanisms related to EFs, our findings suggest that a high-fitness lifestyle, ie, higher PA or lower SB time, might facilitate cognitive health in the older population. However, future research should comprehensively examine executive functioning and its mechanisms further to understand exercise–cognition relationships in the older population.

Despite the effects on TMS and TPST of older adults with specific lifestyles, no significant differences were observed in some indices of the TOL task—TCS, TIT, and TET— between the four activity lifestyles. TCS, the index of move-related performance in the TOL task, indicates the ability to solve more complex problems and is strongly associated with working memory.61 Therefore, our findings implied no difference in working memory between different lifestyles. The TIT and TET indices are speed-related TOL task scores, but they represent different abilities. TIT is regarded as the ability to govern and control executive planning, whereas TET represents the efficiency of executive planning.62 Our findings are partially consistent with the previous study,63 which did not find an association between weekly PA and TET of the TOL task for drivers aged 61–81 years, supporting our finding of a lack of significant differences in speed-related performance in TOL between the four activity lifestyles.

In addition to the TOL task, we used the Stroop test to investigate the combined effect of PA and SB on the core EF, inhibitory control. The Stroop test consisted of assessing inhibitory control and information processing speed. Our findings revealed no significant differences in any conditions of the Stroop test between the four activity lifestyles. This result is similar to the findings from Maasakkers et al (2020), who also reported no relationship between SB time and global CF. However, a recent study observed that the inhibitory control performance of participants involved in high levels of moderate-vigorous PA or low SB time is better.39 These discrepancies may arise due to differences in the sample populations and methodologies used in these studies. We mainly targeted a community sample of older adults, whereas they focused on young and middle-aged adults.39 Furthermore, our work specifically used a modified Stroop task with five task conditions to investigate the relationship between PA, SB, and EFs, unlike Linhares and colleagues’ work,39 which used traditional Stroop with only congruent and incongruent conditions. Future research should focus on a sample of healthy community-dwelling older adults and use other tasks related to inhibitory control to provide updated knowledge on the benefits of PA for the executive functioning of older adults with different activity lifestyles.

While this study is the first to explore the combined effects of PA and SB on core and higher-order EFs, several limitations should be acknowledged. The cross-sectional design limits our ability to infer causality; future longitudinal or intervention-based studies are needed to establish directionality between PA, SB, and EFs. Although we accounted for confounding variables such as age, height, BMI, and intelligence indices, the sample size, while respectable, may limit generalizability. Larger, more diverse cohorts are necessary to enhance external validity. Additionally, reliance on self-reported PA and SB measures introduces potential bias, and future studies should use objective tools like accelerometers. Advanced statistical methods, such as mixed-effects models, should also be considered to account for individual differences and correlations between variables. Lastly, to further elucidate the underlying mechanisms linking PA, SB, and EFs, advanced neuroelectric and neuroimaging techniques, such as electroencephalography (EEG) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), along with biomarkers like brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), could be employed. Incorporating these neuroimaging and biological measures in future studies will offer deeper insights into how PA and SB influence cognitive function in older adults, providing a more comprehensive understanding of these relationships.

Conclusion

The findings of this study highlighted that older adults who engage in higher PA and lower SB performed better in the TOL task (as quantified by the TMS and TPST) than individuals with lower PA and higher SB. This emphasizes that a fit lifestyle enhances the quality and efficiency of executive planning in older individuals. This study also observed that the TMS performance of older adults with lower SB time, regardless of PA expenditure, was better than that of individuals with lower PA and higher SB, suggesting that reduced SB time, even with low PA, can positively influence the quality of executive planning in the older population. Our findings confirm that older adults’ high-order EFs (the TOL task), rather than core EFs (the Stroop test), respond sensitively to higher PA or lower SB time—moving more or sitting less. However, further interventional and longitudinal studies are needed to establish a causal relationship between PA, SB, and core and high-order EFs.

Research Ethics and Consent Statements

We confirm that this study complies with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Acknowledgments

This work was financially supported by the National Science and Technology Council, Taiwan (NSTC 111-2410-H-039-012, 112-2410-H-007-108, 113-2628-H-007-005-MY3).

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

1. Alwin DF, Hofer SM. Health and cognition in aging research. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2011;66 Suppl 1(Suppl 1):i9–16. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbr051

2. Castora-Binkley M, Peronto CL, Edwards JD, Small BJ. A longitudinal analysis of the influence of race on cognitive performance. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2015;70(4):512–518. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbt112

3. World Health Organization. Dementia: A Public Health Priority. 2012:112

4. Brookmeyer R, Johnson E, Ziegler-Graham K, Arrighi HM. Forecasting the global burden of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer's Dement. 2007;3(3):186–191. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2007.04.381

5. Prince M, Bryce R, Albanese E, Wimo A, Ribeiro W, Ferri CP. The global prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimers Dis Dement. 2013;9(1):63–75. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2012.11.007

6. de Bruijn RF, Bos MJ, Portegies ML, et al. The potential for prevention of dementia across two decades: the prospective, population-based Rotterdam study. BMC Med. 2015;13:132. doi:10.1186/s12916-015-0377-5

7. Wimo A, Jönsson L, Bond J, Prince M, Winblad B. The worldwide economic impact of dementia 2010. Alzheimers Dement. 2013;9(1):1–11. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2012.11.006

8. Lu Z, Harris TB, Shiroma EJ, Leung J, Kwok T. Patterns of physical activity and sedentary behavior for older adults with Alzheimer’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, and cognitively normal in Hong Kong. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018;66(4):1453–1462. doi:10.3233/jad-180805

9. Solis-Urra P, Plaza-Diaz J, Álvarez-Mercado AI, et al. The mediation effect of self-report physical activity patterns in the relationship between educational level and cognitive impairment in elderly: a cross-sectional analysis of Chilean health national survey 2016-2017. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(8):2619. doi:10.3390/ijerph17082619

10. Barnes DE, Yaffe K. The projected effect of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer’s disease prevalence. Lancet Neurol. 2011;10(9):819–828. doi:10.1016/s1474-4422(11)70072-2

11. Deckers K, van Boxtel MP, Schiepers OJ, et al. Target risk factors for dementia prevention: a systematic review and Delphi consensus study on the evidence from observational studies. J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2015;30(3):234–246. doi:10.1002/gps.4245

12. Gheysen F, Poppe L, DeSmet A, et al. Physical activity to improve cognition in older adults: can physical activity programs enriched with cognitive challenges enhance the effects? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2018;15(1):63. doi:10.1186/s12966-018-0697-x

13. Banich MT. Executive function: the search for an integrated account. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2009;18(2):89–94. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01615.x

14. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ”frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000;41(1):49–100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

15. Romine CB, Reynolds CR. A model of the development of frontal lobe functioning: findings from a meta-analysis. Appl Neuropsychol. 2005;12(4):190–201. doi:10.1207/s15324826an1204_2

16. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135–168. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

17. Tomporowski PD, McCullick B, Pendleton DM, Pesce C. Exercise and children’s cognition: the role of exercise characteristics and a place for metacognition. J Sport Health Sci. 2015;4(1):47–55. doi:10.1016/j.jshs.2014.09.003

18. Northey JM, Cherbuin N, Pumpa KL, Smee DJ, Rattray B. Exercise interventions for cognitive function in adults older than 50: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Br J Sports Med. 2018;52(3):154–160. doi:10.1136/bjsports-2016-096587

19. Blondell SJ, Hammersley-Mather R, Veerman JL. Does physical activity prevent cognitive decline and dementia?: a systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:510. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-14-510

20. Smiley-Oyen AL, Lowry KA, Francois SJ, Kohut ML, Ekkekakis P. Exercise, fitness, and neurocognitive function in older adults: the ”selective improvement” and ”cardiovascular fitness” hypotheses. Ann Behav Med. 2008;36(3):280–291. doi:10.1007/s12160-008-9064-5

21. Rathore A, Lom B. The effects of chronic and acute physical activity on working memory performance in healthy participants: a systematic review with meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Syst Rev. 2017;6(1):124. doi:10.1186/s13643-017-0514-7

22. Themanson JR, Pontifex MB, Hillman CH. Fitness and action monitoring: evidence for improved cognitive flexibility in young adults. Neuroscience. 2008;157(2):319–328. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.09.014

23. Chen FT, Etnier JL, Chan KH, Chiu PK, Hung TM, Chang YK. Effects of exercise training interventions on executive function in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2020;50(8):1451–1467. doi:10.1007/s40279-020-01292-x

24. Xu L, Gu H, Cai X, et al. The effects of exercise for cognitive function in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2023;20(2). doi:10.3390/ijerph20021088

25. Kumar N, Singh M, Sood S, et al. Effect of acute moderate exercise on cognitive P300 in persons having sedentary lifestyles. Int J Appl Basic Med Res. 2012;2(1):67–69. doi:10.4103/2229-516x.96813

26. Wu ZJ, Wang ZY, Hu BQ, et al. Relationships of accelerometer-based measured objective physical activity and sedentary behaviour with cognitive function: a comparative cross-sectional study of China’s elderly population. BMC Geriatr. 2020;20(1):149. doi:10.1186/s12877-020-01521-y

27. Amagasa S, Inoue S, Murayama H, et al. Associations of sedentary and physically-active behaviors with cognitive-function decline in community-dwelling older adults: compositional data analysis from the NEIGE study. J Epidemiol. 2020;30(11):503–508. doi:10.2188/jea.JE20190141

28. Blair SN. Physical inactivity: the biggest public health problem of the 21st century. Br J Sports Med. 2009;43(1):1–2.

29. Falck RS, Davis JC, Liu-Ambrose T. What is the association between sedentary behaviour and cognitive function? A systematic review. Br J Sports Med. 2017;51(10):800–811. doi:10.1136/bjsports-2015-095551

30. Beunza JJ, Martínez-González MA, Ebrahim S, et al. Sedentary behaviors and the risk of incident hypertension: the SUN Cohort. Am J Hypertens. 2007;20(11):1156–1162. doi:10.1016/j.amjhyper.2007.06.007

31. Wilmot EG, Edwardson CL, Achana FA, et al. Sedentary time in adults and the association with diabetes, cardiovascular disease and death: systematic review and meta-analysis. Diabetologia. 2012;55(11):2895–2905. doi:10.1007/s00125-012-2677-z

32. Cooper AR, Goodman A, Page AS, et al. Objectively measured physical activity and sedentary time in youth: the International children’s accelerometry database (ICAD). Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2015;12:113. doi:10.1186/s12966-015-0274-5

33. Maasakkers CM, Claassen J, Gardiner PA, et al. The association of sedentary behaviour and cognitive function in people without dementia: a coordinated analysis across five cohort studies from COSMIC. Sports Med. 2020;50(2):403–413. doi:10.1007/s40279-019-01186-7

34. Gomez-Pinilla F, Vaynman S, Ying Z. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor functions as a metabotrophin to mediate the effects of exercise on cognition. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28(11):2278–2287. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06524.x

35. Gomez-Pinilla F, Hillman C. The influence of exercise on cognitive abilities. Compr Physiol. 2013;3(1):403–428. doi:10.1002/cphy.c110063

36. Ku PW, Liu YT, Lo MK, Chen LJ, Stubbs B. Higher levels of objectively measured sedentary behavior is associated with worse cognitive ability: two-year follow-up study in community-dwelling older adults. Exp Gerontol. 2017;99:110–114. doi:10.1016/j.exger.2017.09.014

37. Bakrania K, Edwardson CL, Khunti K, Bandelow S, Davies MJ, Yates T. Associations between sedentary behaviors and cognitive function: cross-sectional and prospective findings from the UK biobank. Am J Epidemiol. 2018;187(3):441–454. doi:10.1093/aje/kwx273

38. Zeng X, Cai L, Wong SH, et al. Association of sedentary time and physical activity with executive function among children. Acad Pediatr. 2021;21(1):63–69. doi:10.1016/j.acap.2020.02.027

39. Linhares M, Oliveira GTA, Cabral DAR, et al. Sit less and move more! A cross-sectional study of the associations between physical activity and sedentary behaviors with inhibitory control in Brazilian adults. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2024;73:102643. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2024.102643

40. Liou YM, Jwo CJ, Yao KG, Chiang L-C, Huang L-H. Selection of appropriate Chinese terms to represent intensity and types of physical activity terms for use in the Taiwan version of IPAQ. J Nurs Res. 2008;16(4):252–263. doi:10.1097/01.JNR.0000387313.20386.0a

41. Ekelund U, Steene-Johannessen J, Brown WJ, et al. Does physical activity attenuate, or even eliminate, the detrimental association of sitting time with mortality? A harmonised meta-analysis of data from more than 1 million men and women. Lancet. 2016;388(10051):1302–1310. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(16)30370-1

42. Rikli RE, Jones CJ. Development and validation of criterion-referenced clinically relevant fitness standards for maintaining physical independence in later years. Gerontol. 2013;53(2):255–267. doi:10.1093/geront/gns071

43. Braghieri HA, Kanegusuku H, Corso SD, et al. Validity and reliability of 2-min step test in patients with symptomatic peripheral artery disease. J Vasc Nurs. 2021;39(2):33–38. doi:10.1016/j.jvn.2021.02.004

44. Pedrosa R, Holanda G. Correlation between the walk, 2-minute step and TUG tests among hypertensive older women. Braz J Phys Ther. 2009;13:252–256. doi:10.1590/S1413-35552009005000030

45. Nogueira MA, Almeida TDN, Andrade GS, et al. Reliability and accuracy of 2-minute step test in active and sedentary lean adults. J Manipulative Physiol Ther. 2021;44(2):120–127. doi:10.1016/j.jmpt.2020.07.013

46. Culbertson WC, Moberg PJ, Duda JE, Stern MB, Weintraub D. Assessing the executive function deficits of patients with Parkinson’s disease: utility of the Tower of London-Drexel. Assessment. 2004;11(1):27–39. doi:10.1177/1073191103258590

47. Chang YK, Ku PW, Tomporowski PD, Chen FT, Huang CC. Effects of acute resistance exercise on late-middle-age adults’ goal planning. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2012;44(9):1773–1779. doi:10.1249/MSS.0b013e3182574e0b

48. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18(6):643. doi:10.1037/h0054651

49. Chang YK, Tsai CL, Huang CC, Wang CC, Chu IH. Effects of acute resistance exercise on cognition in late middle-aged adults: general or specific cognitive improvement? J Sci Med Sport. 2014;17(1):51–55. doi:10.1016/j.jsams.2013.02.007

50. Kuo SY, Chen YT, Chang YK, Lee PH, Liu MJ, Chen SR. Influence of internet addiction on executive function and learning attention in Taiwanese school-aged children. Perspectives in Psychiatric Care. 2018;54(4):495–500. doi:10.1111/ppc.12254

51. Chang YK, Huang CJ, Chen KF, Hung TM. Physical activity and working memory in healthy older adults: an ERP study. Psychophysiology. 2013;50(11):1174–1182. doi:10.1111/psyp.12089

52. Berg W, Byrd D. The Tower of London spatial problem-solving task: enhancing clinical and research implementation. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2002;24(5):586–604. doi:10.1076/jcen.24.5.586.1006

53. Ferrari G, Marques A, Barreira TV, et al. Accelerometer-measured daily step counts and adiposity indicators among Latin American adults: a multi-country study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(9). doi:10.3390/ijerph18094641

54. Falck RS, Landry GJ, Best JR, Davis JC, Chiu BK, Liu-Ambrose T. Cross-sectional relationships of physical activity and sedentary behavior with cognitive function in older adults with probable mild cognitive impairment. Phys Ther. 2017;97(10):975–984. doi:10.1093/ptj/pzx074

55. Olanrewaju O, Stockwell S, Stubbs B, Smith L. Sedentary behaviours, cognitive function, and possible mechanisms in older adults: a systematic review. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2020;32(6):969–984. doi:10.1007/s40520-019-01457-3

56. Zlatar ZZ, Hays CC, Mestre Z, et al. Dose-dependent association of accelerometer-measured physical activity and sedentary time with brain perfusion in aging. Exp Gerontol. 2019;125:110679. doi:10.1016/j.exger.2019.110679

57. Carter SE, Draijer R, Holder SM, Brown L, Thijssen DHJ, Hopkins ND. Regular walking breaks prevent the decline in cerebral blood flow associated with prolonged sitting. J Appl Physiol. 2018;125(3):790–798. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00310.2018

58. Chen FT, Hopman RJ, Huang CJ, et al. The effect of exercise training on brain structure and function in older adults: a systematic review based on evidence from randomized control trials. J Clin Med. 2020;9(4):914. doi:10.3390/jcm9040914

59. Moreira PA, Matos SMA, Pitanga FJG, et al. Association between sedentary behavior and cognitive performance in middle-aged and elderly adults: cross-sectional results from ELSA-Brasil. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(21):14234. doi:10.3390/ijerph192114234

60. Sun Y, Ma D, Jiang Z, Han Q, Liu Y, Chen G. The causal relationship between physical activity, sedentary behavior and brain cortical structure: a Mendelian randomization study. Cereb Cortex. 2024;34(4). doi:10.1093/cercor/bhae119

61. Asato MR, Sweeney JA, Luna B. Cognitive processes in the development of TOL performance. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(12):2259–2269. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.010

62. Culbertson WC, Zillmer EA. The construct validity of the Tower of London DX as a measure of the executive functioning of ADHD children. Assessment. 1998;5(3):215–226. doi:10.1177/107319119800500302

63. Marmeleira JF, Godinho MB, Fernandes OM. The effects of an exercise program on several abilities associated with driving performance in older adults. Accid Anal Prev. 2009;41(1):90–97. doi:10.1016/j.aap.2008.09.008

Creative Commons License © 2024 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution - Non Commercial (unported, 3.0) License. By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.