Back to Journals » Psychology Research and Behavior Management » Volume 17
The ERP Components of Reward Processing Modulated by Status-Related Social Comparison
Authors Ma H, Zhang B, Liu M, Wu X
Received 2 February 2024
Accepted for publication 1 October 2024
Published 30 October 2024 Volume 2024:17 Pages 3749—3760
DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S456265
Checked for plagiarism Yes
Review by Single anonymous peer review
Peer reviewer comments 2
Editor who approved publication: Dr Igor Elman
Huanke Ma,1,2,* Boyi Zhang,1,2,* Mengjia Liu,1,2 Xin Wu1,2
1Department of Psychology, Xinxiang Medical University, Henan, 453003, People’s Republic of China; 2Center for Cognition, Emotion, and Body (CCEB), Xinxiang Medical University, Xinxiang, Henan, 453003, People’s Republic of China
*These authors contributed equally to this work
Correspondence: Xin Wu, Department of Psychology, Xinxiang Medical University, Henan, 453003, People’s Republic of China, Email [email protected]
Background: Although social status is closely related to income distribution, few studies have focused on social comparisons caused by income distribution based on social status.
Purpose: The neural indices of status-related social comparisons were investigated by modifying the classical social comparison task with the incorporation of event-related potentials (ERPs).
Methods: The study employed a total of 29 subjects (15 females), the status scores of whom were initially obtained through the utilization of classical measurements of objective (7 items) and subjective (2 items) socioeconomic status. Subsequently, the subjects were required to complete a dot-estimation task. To induce status-related and response-related (upward, equal, and downward) social comparisons, subjects were informed that rewards were distributed based on whether their status score or their response was superior to that of a selected competitor.
Results: The behavioral results demonstrated that status-related social comparisons were perceived as more unfair than response-related social comparisons. The ERP results indicated that the cue-P3 amplitude was lower under status-related cues than response-related cues. Additionally, the amplitude of feedback-related negativity was larger under status-related equal comparisons than response-related equal comparisons. Furthermore, the P3 amplitude was larger under status-related upward comparisons relative to response-related upward comparisons.
Conclusion: The findings indicated that status-related comparisons may contribute to the development of unfair consideration (enhanced FRN) and a reduction in task motivations (lowered cue-P3). Additionally, the status-related upward comparison may serve as a significant factor in the onset of relative deprivation (enhanced P3). It would therefore be beneficial to gain further insight into the neural basis of social comparisons.
Keywords: social comparisons, social status, ERP, FRN, P3
Introduction
Social status can be defined as the respect, admiration and importance that an individual earns from society and closely related to the divisions of interests.1–3 The process of social comparison entails individuals evaluating their own interests in relation to those of others, taking into account factors such as abilities, rewards, and social status. This process can be classified into three main categories: upward comparison (compared with others better than us), equal comparison (compared with others similar to us), and downward comparison (compared with others inferior to us).4–6 While social comparisons have been extensively examined through the lens of task responses (eg, reaction time and accuracy),7–9 these comparisons remain largely confined to the domain of responses or abilities. It remains unclear whether status-related comparisons differ from response-related comparisons, in which interests are divided according to social status. Therefore, it is essential to investigate the mechanisms, particularly the neural mechanisms, of status-related social comparisons, as they may provide more insights into the nature of social comparisons.
The status-related comparisons may entail processes pertaining to reward processing, which differentiate them from response-related comparisons. Prior research indicates that the reward division is perceived as unfair when individuals allocate greater financial compensation to individuals with the same identity, as opposed to those with different identities.10–12 Given that rewards are determined in accordance with status-related identity under status-related comparisons, it is possible that the divisions may be perceived as more unfair under status-related comparisons than under response-related comparisons. Furthermore, research has demonstrated that relative deprivation can be elicited by unfair upward comparisons.13,14 Given that status-related comparisons are perceived as unfair, it seems reasonable to hypothesize that relative deprivation may be induced by status-related upward comparison. In addition, social comparisons play an important role in motivation, enabling individuals to regulate their behaviors in order to obtain rewards.15–17 However, it has been found that the motivational functions can be reduced when rewards cannot provide information for behavioral regulations.18,19 From this perspective, status-related comparisons may have a detrimental effect on motivation, as they do not provide any valid information that can be used to regulate current behavior.
The aforementioned evidence suggests that status-related comparisons may be associated with greater unfairness and diminished motivational functions in comparison to response-related comparisons. Moreover, the relative deprivation may be more pronounced when a status-related upward comparison is made than when a response-related upward comparison is undertaken. In the absence of direct neural evidence to support these opinions, a dot-estimation task was used to investigate the neural mechanisms of status-related comparisons with event-related potentials (ERP) considering that this task has been widely used to investigate the neural basis of the response-related comparisons.7,20–23
The dot-estimation task is widely to investigate the neural basis of social comparison, as evidenced by previous studies.7,20–23 In the dot-estimation task, participants were initially requested to indicate whether the dots represented a quantity that was less than or greater than a specified number (eg, 24) within a given image. Subsequently, they were provided with feedback that informed them about the monetary rewards of both themselves and other participants, with the aim of inducing upward, equal, and downward comparisons.7,20 In contrast with the conventional dot-estimation task, the subjects were initially requested to evaluate their social status by utilizing the established measurements of objective and subjective socioeconomic status,24–27 and subsequently, to undertake the dot-estimation task.7,20 The subjects were informed that the experimenters would distribute the rewards (40 yuan) between them and another individual based on their respective social status scores (status-related cues) and performances on the dot-estimation task (response-related cues). The social comparisons were induced by offering rewards in accordance with the following comparisons: upward (you 10 vs anther 30), equal (you 20 vs another 20), and downward (you 30 vs another 10).
The ERP results indicated that the social comparisons are associated with the ERP components of feedback-related negativity (FRN), P3, and the late positivity potential (LPP),7,23 which could prove beneficial in elucidating the distinction between status-related and response-related social comparisons. The FRN is a negative deflection in the frontocentral regions, with a peak in the time window of 200–300 ms.28,29 In social comparisons, the FRN amplitude was found to be modulated by the feedback valence, with the upward comparison (negative outcomes) eliciting a larger FRN amplitude than the downward comparison (positive outcomes).8,9,30 However, previous studies have also identified a relationship between FRN and fairness considerations, with the FRN amplitude being higher in response to unfair than fair outcomes.31–33 Despite our initial assumption that status-related comparisons would be perceived as more unfair than response-related comparisons, the FRN amplitude may be larger in response to a status-related downward comparison (equal outcome) than a response-related downward comparison (equal outcome), given that the FRN amplitude can also be enhanced by upward comparisons.
P3 is also a significant ERP component associated with social comparison, which represents a positive deflection in the parietal regions with a peak occurring at approximately 300–500ms.9,34 It has been demonstrated that individuals allocate a greater proportion of their attentional resources to downward comparisons than to upward comparisons, as evidenced by the enhanced P3 amplitude observed in the context of downward comparisons.35–37 Nevertheless, it has been proposed that the experience of relative deprivation may be accompanied by negative emotional states, including anger and resentment.38–40 Prior research has demonstrated that individuals allocate greater attentional resources to angry stimuli in order to cope with potential threats, which is evidenced by higher P3 amplitudes in response to angry stimuli compared to positive stimuli (eg, happiness).41–43 Therefore, the P3 amplitude may be enhanced by status-related upward comparison due to relative deprivation, which could potentially reduce the discrepancy in P3 between upward and downward comparisons under status-related conditions, while simultaneously increasing the divergence between status-related and response-related upward comparisons.
Based on the aforementioned reasons, we postulated that individuals might perceive status-related comparisons as more unfair than response-related comparisons. Additionally, we hypothesized that the FRN amplitude elicited by an upward (equal) comparison under a status-related comparison might be larger than under a response-related comparison. Similarly, we predicted that the P3 amplitude elicited by an upward comparison under a status-related comparison might be larger than under a response-related comparison. Furthermore, research has demonstrated that the cue-P3 has a larger amplitude when cues are perceived as motivational.44,45 Given that response-related rewards can convey more motivational information than status-related, it can be postulated that the cue-P3 amplitude may be larger when response-related cues are presented than when status-related cues are presented.
Method
Experiment Design and Subjects
In this study, a 2 (reward division: status-related and response-related) × 3 (social comparison: upward, equal, and downward) within-subject design was used. The minimum sample size was calculated using G*power (V3.1.9.4) with the following parameters: α = 0.05, effect size f = 0.25, and power = 0.8. Based on these parameters, 24 participants were required to meet the minimum sample size. Thus, 30 right-handed participants (15 females) from Xinxiang Medical University were included in this study. All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations or declaration of Helsinki. All of the subjects signed informed consent forms and were approved by the ethics committee of the Xinxiang Medical University. Data from one participant were excluded because of excessive artifacts in EEG recordings (See Table S1).
Experiment Procedure
In this study, participants were first asked to fill out a questionnaire of social status and then completed the dot-estimation task. Moreover, for the sake of inducing social comparison, the subjects were told that the experimenters would determine whether they could acquire extra rewards according to their social status or performance relative to the other subjects.
To measure social status, the participants were asked to complete the objective socioeconomic status (OSES) and subjective socioeconomic status (SSES). According to previous studies,24,46 the OSES was used to measure 5 scores of social status based on individual’s household income, parental education, and occupation. The SSES was measured using the MacArthur Ladder,25,26 where individuals had to imagine that the 10-rung ladder represented people’s position in Chinese society and their school.
After social status measurements, the participants completed the social comparison task. In the social comparison task, the stimulating font is white and the background is black. The stimulus set consists of ten dot pictures with a very number, and the participants were required to judge whether the points were odd or even and press the corresponding key. Prior to the experiment, every participant was informed that they would concurrently complete a basic numerical judgment task with the other participant in the adjacent laboratory, and that he/she could earn extra rewards that were dependent on their performance or one of the items on the social status questionnaire relative to the other subjects in each trial. The other person was a faux subject, meaning that both his/her social standing and performance were preset. At the conclusion of the trial, they were given additional funds in addition to the original show-up fee.
Figure 1 displays the time flow of each experiment. In the beginning, the fixation point showed up in the middle of the screen for 0.5 seconds. Later that time, various amounts of black dots were displayed for 2s, where the subjects were asked to judge whether the points were odd or even and to press the corresponding keystroke. After selecting (0.8–1.5s) and a short delay (0.5–0.7s), a division of status/response was observed, indicating that an extra reward was allocated accordingly. The award feedback screen told the subject about the reward amount distribution plan after a brief pause of 0.5–0.7 seconds. Lastly, they had to rate their level of satisfaction, and press “1” means very dissatisfaction to “5” means very satisfaction. In addition, considering that unfair consideration could be induced under status-related social comparisons, the fairness score was measured using a 4-point scale (1= very unfair, 2 = unfair, 3 = fair, and 4 = very fair) after completing the social comparison task.
![]() |
Figure 1 The time course of each trial in social comparison task. |
The experiment was split into two phases: practice and test, with the purpose of acquainting the volunteers with the task’s procedure and the answer buttons. There were six types of trials. In the practice test, each trial was repeated 3 times. The official examination phase was comprised 216 trials, with each trial repeated 36 times.
EEG Data Collection and Preprocessing
64 Ag–AgCl scalp locations had their brain electrical activity monitored using an elastic cap (Neuro Scan Product). The left supraorbital and infraorbital rows of electrodes were used to record eye blinks. Two external canthi, 1.5 cm lateral to each other, were the sites of a row of electrodes used to capture the horizontal electro-oculogram (EOG). The electrode recordings’ rows were all cited online. They were offline and re-referenced to the mean of the data for the left and right mastoids. Feedback-locked ERPs were obtained by extracting the division EEG epochs, which were 1000 ms, and the social comparison ERP epochs, which were 1500 ms (with a 200 ms pre feedback baseline). A 0.01–30 hz band-pass filter was used to amplify the biosignals, and 500 hz/channel of continuous sampling was done for off-line analysis. Blinks and eye movements were rejected offline as eye movement artifacts.
Statistical Analysis
According to our hypotheses, we analyzed cue-P3 in the cue stage, and FRN and P3 in the social comparison stage. The cue-P3 mean amplitudes were calculated in the 320–420ms time window across the medial parietal electrodes (Pz and POz) according to previous studies.47,48 Based on previous studies,49–51 the FRN was analyzed by first calculating the difference wave between the status-related equal comparison and response-related equal comparison, and then the peak latency (258ms) of the FRN was obtained in the time windows (228–288ms); finally, the FRN mean amplitudes were calculated in the time window across the medial frontocentral electrodes (Fz and FCz). P3 was analyzed in the time windows of 330–390ms across the parietal electrodes (P1 & Pz) based on the collapsed localizer (See Figure S1) as previous studies.52–54 In addition to the FRN and P3, the late positivity potential (LPP) was also analyzed in the 450–550ms time window across the parietal electrodes (P1 & Pz) according to the collapsed localizer (See Figure S1) considering the roles of the LPP in the social comparison.55,56
The statistical analysis of the behavioral and ERP data was conducted with SPSS software (version 26, SPSS Inc)., with a statistical threshold of 0.05 for all analyses. Specifically, a 2 (reward division: status-related and response-related) × 3 (social comparison: upward, equal, and downward) two-way ANOVA of the within-subject design was used to analyze the satisfaction scores and mean amplitudes of the FRN, P3, and LPP. In addition, a paired T test was used to analyze the mean amplitude of cue-P3 and the fairness score.
Results
Behavioural Results
The results demonstrated that the fairness score of the status-related division (2.0357 ± 0.141) was lower than that of the response-related division (3.3214 ± 0.155) [t (1, 28) = −5.229, p < 0.001, d= −0.988]. The repeated-measures ANOVA findings for the satisfaction scores indicated that the main effects of both reward division [F (1, 28) = 10.606, p = 0.003, η2 = 0.275] and social comparison [F (2, 56) = 92.248, p < 0 0.001, η2 = 0.767] were significant. Moreover, the interaction effect was also significant [F (2, 54) = 5.817, p = 0.005, η2 = 0.1172]. The simple effect test demonstrated that the satisfaction score under the status-related division was lower than that under the response-related division when downward social comparison was induced [F (1, 28) = 13.519, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.326], whereas satisfaction scores were not significantly different between status-related and response-related divisions when upward and equal social comparisons were induced.
ERP Results
The Results of Cue-P3
The results showed that the response-related cue elicited a larger cue-P3 amplitude (4.221 ± 3.868) than the status-related cue (3.0312 ± 4.833) [t (1, 28) = −2.181, p < 0.05, d = −0.405] (See Figure 2).
![]() |
Figure 2 Grand-averaged ERPs and the difference wave (status-related cues minus response-related cues) at Pz and POz, as well as the topographic maps of the cue-P3 in the time window 320–420ms. |
The Results of the FRN
The results showed that the main effects of reward division and the social comparison were not significant, while the interaction between reward division and the social comparison was significant [F (2, 56) = 3.811, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.120]. The simple effect test showed that there were no significantly different between status-related and response-related upward comparison [F (1, 28) = 1.839, p = 0.186, η2 = 0.062], as well as status-related and response-related downward comparison [F (1, 28) = 0.182, p = 0.673, η2 = 0.006], but the status-related equal comparison elicited a larger FRN than the response-related equal comparison [F (1, 28) = 6.371, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.185] (See Figure 3).
The Results of the P3
The results showed that the main effects of reward division and the social comparison were not significant, while the interaction between reward division and the social comparison was significant [F (2, 56) = 5.654, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.168]. The simple effect test showed that the status-related upward comparison elicited a larger P3 than the response-related upward comparison [F (1, 28) = 5.728, p < 0.05, η2 =0.170], while there was no significantly different between status-related and response-related comparisons when downward [F (1, 28) = 3.550, p = 0.070, η2 = 0.113] and equal [F (1, 28) = 2.924, p = 0.098, η2 = 0.095] comparisons were induced. Moreover, there were significant differences among the response-related upward, equal, and downward comparisons [F (2, 28) = 5.439, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.287], but there was no significant difference among status-related upward, equal, and downward comparisons [F (2,28) = 0.541, p = 0.588, η2 =0.039]. The paired T test showed that the P3 amplitude was significantly or marginally significantly lower under response-related upward comparison than equal comparison (p = 0.008) and downward comparison (p = 0.052), but no significant difference between downward and equal comparisons (See Figures 4 and 5).
![]() |
Figure 4 Grand-averaged ERPs at P1 and Pz for the social comparisons. |
![]() |
Figure 5 The topographic maps for the P3 and LPP under the social comparisons in the time windows of 336–396ms and 450–550ms respectively. |
The Results of the LPP
Repeated measures ANOVA revealed that the main effect of division was not found, but the main effect of social comparison was found [F (2, 56) = 6.105, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.179]. We also not find interaction effect of division and social comparison. The results of the post hoc test show that the upward comparison induces a smaller LPP than the equal and downward comparison. There was no difference between equal and downward, whether they were status-related or response-related (See Figures 4 and 5).
Discussion
In this study, electrophysiological indices of status-related social comparisons were investigated using a modified social comparison task combining with the ERP technique. In accordance with our hypotheses, the behavioral results showed that the individuals reported that the status-related comparisons were more unfair than the response-related comparisons. The ERP results showed that the status-related cue elicited a larger cue-P3 amplitude than the response-related cue; the status-related upward comparison caused a higher P3 amplitude than the response-related upward comparison; the status-related equal comparison elicited a larger FRN amplitude than the response-related equal comparison. Except for these results, we found that the upward comparison elicited a smaller LPP amplitude than both the equal and downward comparisons.
Our results revealed that the fairness score under the status-related comparison was lower than under the response-related comparison; the FRN amplitude was larger under status-related equal comparisons than under response-related equal comparisons. It was confirmed that the FRN amplitude can be moderated by the reward-division fairness, which showed that the FRN amplitude is larger under the unfair division than the fair division.28,57 Furthermore, previous studies also define the reward division as unfairness when individuals give the persons with same identity more money than others with different identity.10–12 Our results implied that unfairness could be observed by the larger FRN amplitude under the status-related equal comparison than the response-related equal comparison. Considering that the FRN amplitude can be also enhanced by other response-related comparisons based on previous studies,58–60 it might be difficult to be observe the difference between response-related and status-related upward/downward comparisons because of the confusion of unfairness under the status-related division. Thus, it might be an important way to observe the unfairness using the enhanced FRN amplitude under the status-related equal comparison relative to the response-related equal comparison.
Our results also found that the P3 amplitude elicited by the upward comparison was larger under the status-related comparison than response-related comparison. It has been suggested that unfair upward comparison can induce relative deprivation accompanied by anger and resentful.38–40 Furthermore, previous studies also found that P3 amplitude is larger under anger than happiness due to more attentional resources are assigned to the angry stimuli to cope with potential threat.41–43,61 Considering that the status-related comparisons are more unfair than the response-related comparisons, the enhanced P3 under the status-related upward comparison might be related to the relative deprivation induced by the unfair upward comparison. Similar to the anger, the enhanced P3 amplitude also implied that the attentional system might also assign more resources to the relative deprivation induced by the status-related upward comparison.
A substantial body of research indicates that relative deprivation is significantly associated with a range of adverse outcomes, including aggressive behavior, antisocial conduct and criminality.39,62,63 The theory of relative deprivation posits that relative deprivation can be induced and may instigate aggressive behavior when individuals perceive that others are better off and that their own disadvantage is undeserved.38,64 It has been demonstrated that aggressive behavior can be moderated by cognitive control, indicating that the aggressive behavior could be successfully inhibited if individuals possess sufficient cognitive resources to execute the cognitive control.65–67 Our results indicate that the relative deprivation may require a significant number of cognitive resources due to the enhanced P3 amplitude observed in status-related upward comparisons relative to response-related upward comparisons. Therefore, it can be posited that the cognitive resources available may be insufficient to inhibit aggressive behaviors when the relative deprivation is induced by an unfair upward comparison.
Our results also demonstrated that the amplitude of the cue-P3 was diminished in response to status-related cues in comparison to response-related cues. The cue-P3 is responsive to incentive cues and is capable of conveying monetary contingencies under both loss and gain relative to neutral cues.44,68–70 It has been proposed that an increased cue-P3 amplitude indicates that individuals have allocated a greater number of attentional resources to reward-predicting stimuli, which can stimulate the motivation to engage in reward-seeking behaviors.69,70 Consequently, the reduced cue-P3 amplitude may suggest that status-related cues possess diminished motivational functions in comparison to response-related cues. In other words, the response-related cues may exert a more pronounced motivational influence on individuals to engage in reward-seeking behaviors than the status-related cues.
This approach offers an effective method of influencing behaviors in a socializing context, whereby individuals are rewarded contingent on their ability to meet the behavioral standards valued by significant others (eg parents, teachers, elders).71–74 Although this perspective implies that it is an important way to motivate behaviors by using the response-related division of reward, social status plays an important role in satisfying the psychological need for competence.1–3,75 It may therefore be inevitable to distribute rewards on the basis of social status. It is important to note, however, that the motivation to receive a reward may be reduced when social comparisons are made on the basis of social status. Moreover, individuals may experience relative deprivation when they perceive status-related upward comparison, which could potentially result in adverse outcomes, including anger, resentment, and aggressive behaviors.38–40 It is therefore important to strike a balance between response-related (eg performance-based rewards) and status-related divisions (eg salary rank) in practice, such as in management, education and other fields, where rewards are used to shape behaviors.
Conclusions and Limitations
The present study investigated the ERP components of status-based social comparisons using the modified dot-estimation task. The findings indicated that status-related comparisons were perceived as more unfair than response-related comparisons. This was evidenced by the observation of a higher FRN amplitude in response to status-based equal comparisons relative to response-based equal comparisons. Furthermore, the motivational impact of the rewards was diminished in the context of status-related comparisons, potentially linked to the reduced cue-P3 amplitude observed in status-related cues relative to response-related cues. Additionally, the P3 amplitude was elevated in the context of status-related upward comparisons compared to response-related upward comparisons, suggesting that individuals may have experienced a sense of relative deprivation in the context of an unfair upward comparison.
The findings presented here contribute to our understanding of the neural basis of status-related comparisons. However, it is also necessary to address some of the limitations of the present study in future research. Firstly, considering that status-related social comparisons were triggered by classical measurements of objective and subjective socioeconomic status, it was difficult to determine whether the responses were correct. Inconsistent with the classical dot-estimation task,7,20,23 there was no correct or incorrect information in the feedback. Thus, it is necessary to study the electrophysiological indices of status-related social comparisons, in which social status can also be evaluated as right or wrong.
Secondly, our findings showed that the LPP amplitudes during upward comparisons were the smallest, whereas the LPP amplitudes were not different between equal and downward comparisons. Previous experiments have shown that LPP may reflect the detection of stimuli with motivational marker.76 In previous studies, positive stimuli may enhance LPP, which is inconsistent with our experimental results.7,23,54 However, we were unable to provide a reasonable explanation for this finding. One possible explanation is that in the case of downward comparison and equal comparison, it will have a stronger incentive function than in the upward comparison. They reinforced and motivated participants’ feedback during the trial. Another possible reason is that downward and equal comparisons induce more emotion regulation strategies.77,78 Further research is needed to provide a more reasonable explanation for the results of the LPP.
Thirdly, although our findings implied that relative deprivation might be related to the higher P3 amplitude elicited by status-related upward comparisons, but the relative deprivation was not directly measured in the current research. Thus, future research should examine the connection between P3 and relative deprivation, as well as its roles in aggressive behavior, antisocial conduct and criminality.
Data Sharing Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available if necessary.
Acknowledgments
Huanke Ma & Boyi Zhang are co-first authors for this study. This work was supported by the Key Scientific Research Projects of Colleges and Universities of Henan Province (20A190001), and the Humanities and Social Sciences at Universities of Henan Province (2024ZZJH302).
Disclosure
The authors declared that they have no conflicts of interest with respect to the authorship or publication of this article.
References
1. Gregg A, Sedikides C, Pegler A. Self-esteem and social status: dominance theory and hierometer theory. Encycl Evol Psychol Sci. 2018;2018:1–6.
2. Mahadevan N, Gregg AP, Sedikides C. Is self-regard a sociometer or a hierometer? Self-esteem tracks status and inclusion, narcissism tracks status. J Personal Soc Psychol. 2019;116(3):444. doi:10.1037/pspp0000189
3. Mahadevan N, Gregg AP, Sedikides C. Self-esteem as a hierometer: sociometric status is a more potent and proximate predictor of self-esteem than socioeconomic status. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2021;150(12):2613. doi:10.1037/xge0001056
4. Festinger L. A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relation. 1954;7(2):117–140. doi:10.1177/001872675400700202
5. Hakmiller KL. Threat as a determinant of downward comparison. J Experiment Soc Psychol. 1966;1:32–39. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(66)90063-1
6. Wheeler L, Miyake K. Social comparison in everyday life. J Personal Soc Psychol. 1992;62(5):760. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.62.5.760
7. Qiu J, Yu C, Li H, et al. The impact of social comparison on the neural substrates of reward processing: an event-related potential study. Neuroimage. 2010;49(1):956–962. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.025
8. Liu S, Hu X, Mai X. Social distance modulates outcome processing when comparing abilities with others. Psychophysiology. 2021;58(5):e13798. doi:10.1111/psyp.13798
9. Wu Y, Zhang D, Elieson B, Zhou X. Brain potentials in outcome evaluation: when social comparison takes effect. Int J Psychophysiol. 2012;85(2):145–152. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.06.004
10. Volz KG, Kessler T, von Cramon DY. In-group as part of the self: in-group favoritism is mediated by medial prefrontal cortex activation. Social Neurosci. 2009;4(3):244–260. doi:10.1080/17470910802553565
11. Zhang Z, Su H, Li M, Zhao H, Qi C. Effects of Ingroup Identification on Ingroup Favouritism during Fairness Norm Enforcement. Behav Sci. 2022;12(11):415. doi:10.3390/bs12110415
12. Chae H, Banerjee A, Dussauze M, Albeanu DF. Long-range functional loops in the mouse olfactory system and their roles in computing odor identity. Neuron. 2022;110(23):3970–3985. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2022.09.005
13. Wilkinson RG, Pickett KE. The problems of relative deprivation: why some societies do better than others. Soc sci med. 2007;65(9):1965–1978. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.05.041
14. Smith DV, Clithero JA, Boltuck SE, Huettel SA. Functional connectivity with ventromedial prefrontal cortex reflects subjective value for social rewards. Soc Cognit Affective Neurosci. 2014;9(12):2017–2025. doi:10.1093/scan/nsu005
15. Helgeson VS, Mickelson KD. Motives for social comparison. Personal Social Psychol Bullet. 1995;21(11):1200–1209. doi:10.1177/01461672952111008
16. Keltner D, Gross JJ. Functional accounts of emotions. Cognition Emot. 1999;13(5):467–480. doi:10.1080/026999399379140
17. Van de Ven N. Envy and admiration: emotion and motivation following upward social comparison. Cognition Emotion. 2017;31(1):193–200. doi:10.1080/02699931.2015.1087972
18. Deci EL, Koestner R, Ryan RM. A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychol Bull. 1999;125(6):627. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.627
19. Deci EL, Olafsen AH, Ryan RM. Self-determination theory in work organizations: the state of a science. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior. 2017;4(1):19–43. doi:10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032516-113108
20. Fliessbach K, Weber B, Trautner P, et al. Social comparison affects reward-related brain activity in the human ventral striatum. science. 2007;318(5854):1305–1308. doi:10.1126/science.1145876
21. Du X, Zhang M, Wei D, Li W, Zhang Q, Qiu J. The neural circuitry of reward processing in complex social comparison: evidence from an event-related FMRI study. PLoS One. 2013;8(12):e82534. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0082534
22. Du X, Yao L, Sun L, Chen X, Jiang J. Neural mechanisms of social comparison in subthreshold depression. Cereb Cortex. 2024;34(5):bhae222. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhae222
23. Zou F, Li X, Chen F, et al. P2 manifests subjective evaluation of reward processing under social comparison. Front Psychol. 2022;13:817529. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2022.817529
24. Brennan SL, Henry MJ, Nicholson GC, Kotowicz MA, Pasco JA. Socioeconomic status and risk factors for obesity and metabolic disorders in a population-based sample of adult females. Preventive Med. 2009;49(2–3):165–171. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2009.06.021
25. Goodman E, Huang B. Socioeconomic status, depression, and health service utilization among adolescent women. Women Health Issu. 2001;11(5):416–426. doi:10.1016/S1049-3867(01)00077-9
26. Ostrove JM, Adler NE, Kuppermann M, Washington AE. Objective and subjective assessments of socioeconomic status and their relationship to self-rated health in an ethnically diverse sample of pregnant women. Health Psychol. 2000;19(6):613. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.613
27. Guo P, Cui J, Wang Y, Zou F, Wu X, Zhang M. Spontaneous microstates related to effects of low socioeconomic status on neuroticism. Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):15710. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-72590-7
28. Gehring WJ, Willoughby AR. The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science. 2002;295(5563):2279–2282. doi:10.1126/science.1066893
29. Lin H, Liang J. Behavioral and ERP effects of encoded facial expressions on facial identity recognition depend on recognized facial expressions. Psychological Res. 2023;87(5):1590–1606. doi:10.1007/s00426-022-01756-x
30. Luo Y, Feng C, Wu T, et al. Social comparison manifests in event-related potentials. Sci Rep. 2015;5(1):12127. doi:10.1038/srep12127
31. Massi B, Luhmann CC. Fairness influences early signatures of reward-related neural processing. Cognitive, Affective. Behav Neurosci. 2015;15:768–775.
32. Mussel P, Hewig J, Allen JJ, Coles MG, Miltner W. Smiling faces, sometimes they don’t tell the truth: facial expression in the ultimatum game impacts decision making and event‐related potentials. Psychophysiology. 2014;51(4):358–363. doi:10.1111/psyp.12184
33. Chen T, Tang R, Yang X, Peng M, Cai M. Moral transgression modulates fairness considerations in the ultimatum game: evidence from ERP and EEG data. Int J Psychophysiol. 2023;188:1–11. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2023.03.001
34. Hajcak G, Foti D. Significance? Significance! Empirical, methodological, and theoretical connections between the late positive potential and P300 as neural responses to stimulus significance: an integrative review. Psychophysiology. 2020;57(7):e13570. doi:10.1111/psyp.13570
35. Yeung N, Holroyd CB, Cohen JD. ERP correlates of feedback and reward processing in the presence and absence of response choice. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15(5):535–544. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh153
36. Leng Y, Zhou X. Modulation of the brain activity in outcome evaluation by interpersonal relationship: an ERP study. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(2):448–455. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.002
37. Sato A, Yasuda A, Ohira H, et al. Effects of value and reward magnitude on feedback negativity and P300. Neuroreport. 2005;16(4):407–411. doi:10.1097/00001756-200503150-00020
38. Smith HJ, Pettigrew TF, Pippin GM, Bialosiewicz S. Relative deprivation: a theoretical and meta-analytic review. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2012;16(3):203–232. doi:10.1177/1088868311430825
39. Greitemeyer T, Sagioglou C. The impact of personal relative deprivation on aggression over time. J Soc Psychol. 2019;159(6):664–675. doi:10.1080/00224545.2018.1549013
40. Jiang T, Chen Z. Relative deprivation: a mechanism for the ostracism–aggression link. Eur J Social Psychol. 2020;50(2):347–359. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2621
41. Green MJ, Williams LM, Davidson D. Visual scanpaths to threat-related faces in deluded schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2003;119(3):271–285. doi:10.1016/S0165-1781(03)00129-X
42. Stewart JL, Silton RL, Sass SM, et al. Attentional bias to negative emotion as a function of approach and withdrawal anger styles: an ERP investigation. Int J Psychophysiol. 2010;76(1):9–18. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.01.008
43. Shangguan C, Wang X, Li X, Wang Y, Lu J, Li Z. Inhibition and production of anger cost more: evidence from an ERP study on the production and switch of voluntary facial emotional expression. Front Psychol. 2019;10:1276. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01276
44. Goldstein RZ, Cottone LA, Jia Z, Maloney T, Volkow ND, Squires NK. The effect of graded monetary reward on cognitive event-related potentials and behavior in young healthy adults. Int J Psychophysiol. 2006;62(2):272–279. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.05.006
45. Zhang Y, Li Q, Wang Z, Liu X, Zheng Y. Temporal dynamics of reward anticipation in the human brain. Biological Psychology. 2017;128:89–97. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.07.011
46. Benhabib J, Bisin A, Zhu S. The wealth distribution in Bewley economies with capital income risk. J Econ Theory. 2015;159:489–515. doi:10.1016/j.jet.2015.07.013
47. Mento G. The role of the P3 and CNV components in voluntary and automatic temporal orienting: a high spatial-resolution ERP study. Neuropsychologia. 2017;107:31–40. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.10.037
48. Cofresí RU, Piasecki TM, Hajcak G, Bartholow BD. Internal consistency and test–retest reliability of the P3 event‐related potential (ERP) elicited by alcoholic and non‐alcoholic beverage pictures. Psychophysiology. 2022;59(2):e13967. doi:10.1111/psyp.13967
49. Holroyd CB, Coles MG. The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev. 2002;109(4):679. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.4.679
50. Li P, Jia S, Feng T, Liu Q, Suo T, Li H. The influence of the diffusion of responsibility effect on outcome evaluations: electrophysiological evidence from an ERP study. Neuroimage. 2010;52(4):1727–1733. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.275
51. Hauser TU, Iannaccone R, Stämpfli P, et al. The feedback-related negativity (FRN) revisited: new insights into the localization, meaning and network organization. Neuroimage. 2014;84:159–168. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.028
52. Luck SJ, Gaspelin N. How to get statistically significant effects in any ERP experiment (and why you shouldn’t). Psychophysiology. 2017;54(1):146–157. doi:10.1111/psyp.12639
53. Sui J, He X, Golubickis M, Svensson SL, Macrae CN. Electrophysiological correlates of self-prioritization. Consciousness Cognition. 2023;108:103475. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2023.103475
54. Sima J, Ma H, Liu F, et al. Electrophysiological indexes of ingroup bias in a group Stroop task: evidence from an event-related potential study. Behav Brain Res. 2024;464:114931. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2024.114931
55. Wu Y, Zhou Y, van Dijk E, Leliveld MC, Zhou X. Social comparison affects brain responses to fairness in asset division: an ERP study with the ultimatum game. Front Human Neurosci. 2011;5:131. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2011.00131
56. Uusberg H, Peet K, Uusberg A, Akkermann K. Attention biases in preoccupation with body image: an ERP study of the role of social comparison and automaticity when processing body size. Biological Psychology. 2018;135:136–148. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.03.007
57. Peterburs J, Kobza S, Bellebaum C. Feedback delay gradually affects amplitude and valence specificity of the feedback‐related negativity (FRN). Psychophysiology. 2016;53(2):209–215. doi:10.1111/psyp.12560
58. Shenhav A, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD. The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron. 2013;79(2):217–240. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.007
59. Shenhav A, Cohen JD, Botvinick MM. Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and the value of control. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19(10):1286–1291. doi:10.1038/nn.4384
60. Shackman AJ, Maxwell JS, McMenamin BW, Greischar LL, Davidson RJ. Stress potentiates early and attenuates late stages of visual processing. J Neurosci. 2011;31(3):1156–1161. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3384-10.2011
61. Crago RV, Renoult L, Biggart L, Nobes G, Satmarean T, Bowler JO. Physical aggression and attentional bias to angry faces: an event related potential study. Brain Res. 2019;1723:146387. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2019.146387
62. DeCelles KA, Norton MI. Physical and situational inequality on airplanes predicts air rage. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016;113(20):5588–5591. doi:10.1073/pnas.1521727113
63. Greitemeyer T, Sagioglou C. Increasing wealth inequality may increase interpersonal hostility: the relationship between personal relative deprivation and aggression. J Soc Psychol. 2017;157(6):766–776. doi:10.1080/00224545.2017.1288078
64. Smith HJ, Pettigrew TF. The subjective interpretation of inequality: a model of the relative deprivation experience. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2014;8(12):755–765. doi:10.1111/spc3.12151
65. Wilkowski BM, Robinson MD, Troop-Gordon W. How does cognitive control reduce anger and aggression? The role of conflict monitoring and forgiveness processes. J Personal Soc Psychol. 2010;98(5):830. doi:10.1037/a0018962
66. Wilkowski BM, Crowe SE, Ferguson EL. Learning to keep your cool: reducing aggression through the experimental modification of cognitive control. Cognition Emotion. 2015;29(2):251–265. doi:10.1080/02699931.2014.911146
67. Weidler C, Habel U, Wallheinke P, et al. Consequences of prefrontal tDCS on inhibitory control and reactive aggression. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2022;17(1):120–130. doi:10.1093/scan/nsaa158
68. Broyd SJ, Richards HJ, Helps SK, Chronaki G, Bamford S, Sonuga-Barke EJ. An electrophysiological monetary incentive delay (e-MID) task: a way to decompose the different components of neural response to positive and negative monetary reinforcement. J Neurosci Method. 2012;209(1):40–49. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2012.05.015
69. Pfabigan DM, Seidel EM, Sladky R, et al. P300 amplitude variation is related to ventral striatum BOLD response during gain and loss anticipation: an EEG and fMRI experiment. NeuroImage. 2014;96:12–21. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.077
70. Novak KD, Foti D. Teasing apart the anticipatory and consummatory processing of monetary incentives: an event‐related potential study of reward dynamics. Psychophysiology. 2015;52(11):1470–1482. doi:10.1111/psyp.12504
71. McDowell JJ. Matching theory in natural human environments. Behavior Analyst. 1988;11(2):95–109. doi:10.1007/BF03392462
72. Leary MR, Baumeister RF. The nature and function of self-esteem: sociometer theory. Adv Experiment Soc Psychol. 2000;32:1–62.
73. Pyszczynski T, Greenberg J, Solomon S, Arndt J, Schimel J. Why do people need self-esteem? A theoretical and empirical review. Psychol Bull. 2004;130(3):435. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.435
74. Proctor D, Leeder D, Mattick K. The case for faculty development: a realist evaluation. Med Educ. 2020;54(9):832–842. doi:10.1111/medu.14204
75. Ryan RM. Self-Determination Theory: Basic Psychological Needs in Motivation, Development, and Wellness. Guilford Press; 2017.
76. Cunningham WA, Espinet SD, DeYoung CG, Zelazo PD. Attitudes to the right- and left: frontal ERP asymmetries associated with stimulus valence and processing goals. NeuroImage. 2005;28(4):827–834. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.044
77. Hajcak G, Nieuwenhuis S. Reappraisal modulates the electrocortical response to unpleasant pictures. Cognit Affective Behav Neurosci. 2006;6(4):291–297. doi:10.3758/CABN.6.4.291
78. Paul S, Simon D, Kniesche R, Kathmann N, Endrass T. Timing effects of antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation strategies. Biological Psychol. 2013;94(1):136–142. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.05.019
© 2024 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The
full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php
and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution
- Non Commercial (unported, 3.0) License.
By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted
without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly
attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.