Back to Journals » Nature and Science of Sleep » Volume 16

Time-Dependent Effects of Altered Prebedtime Light Exposure in Enclosed Spaces on Sleep Performance Associated with Human States

Authors Xiao J , Chen D, Yu S, Wang H, Sun Y, Wang H, Gou Z , Wang J

Received 9 April 2024

Accepted for publication 1 August 2024

Published 6 August 2024 Volume 2024:16 Pages 1179—1200

DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/NSS.S472988

Checked for plagiarism Yes

Review by Single anonymous peer review

Peer reviewer comments 3

Editor who approved publication: Prof. Dr. Ahmed BaHammam



Jianghao Xiao, Dengkai Chen, Suihuai Yu, Hui Wang, Yiwei Sun, Hanyu Wang, Zhiming Gou, Jingping Wang

Key Laboratory of Industrial Design and Ergonomics, Ministry of Industry and Information Technology, Northwestern Polytechnical University, Xi’an, 710072, People’s Republic of China

Correspondence: Jianghao Xiao, Key Laboratory of Industrial Design and Ergonomics, Ministry of Industry and Information Technology, Northwestern Polytechnical University, 127 Youyi West Road, Xi’an, 710072, People’s Republic of China, Tel +86 15528076860, Email [email protected]

Purpose: Exposure to artificial light influences human performance, which is essential for maintaining healthy work and sleep. However, existing research has not explored the intrinsic links between sleep performance and human states over time under prebedtime light exposure interventions (LEIs).
Methods: To investigate the time-dependent effects of altered prebedtime light exposure, four LEI groupings (#L1 - #L4) and a Time factor (D8, D9, and D10) were chosen for sleep experiments in enclosed spaces. Forty-eight young adults recruited were available for data analysis. Subjective alertness (SA), negative affect (NA), subjective sleep, and objective sleep were measured via the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale, Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, Next-day Self-assessment Sleep Quality, and joint assessment of wrist actigraphy and sleep diaries, respectively. Statistical analysis was used for the effects of light exposure on the human states (corresponding to the SA and NA) and sleep performance, while the process model helped construct the associations between the two.
Results: The statistical effects revealed that the Time had a significant main effect on subjective sleep and changes in prebedtime alertness; the LEI had a significant main effect only on sleep onset latency (SOL). After undergoing altered prebedtime light exposure, the mean SA increased at prebedtime of D9 (p = 0.022) and D10 (p = 0.044); No significant effect on the NA was observed; Mean subjective sleep had a significant increase from D8 to D10. Moreover, five actigraphy-estimated sleep parameters were interrelated. In light of this, a chained pathway relationship was identified. The SOL played a mediating predictor between prebedtime state and objective sleep, which was linked to the awakening state through subjective sleep.
Conclusion: Our study suggests that time-dependent effects of altered prebedtime light exposure on sleep performance are associated with human states at prebedtime and awakening, with implications for its prediction of sleep health.

Keywords: prebedtime light exposure, sleep performance, non-visual effects, alertness, negative affect, sleep onset latency

Introduction

Light is one of the main drivers of circadian system.1 The artificial light at night was an extremely disruptive cue;2,3 for example, nearly half of homes experienced evening lighting detrimental to human sleep.3 Extendedly, the impact of ocular light is categorized as photic and non-photic.4 Rod and cone cells are mainly related to human vision, while the intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs)5 affect non-visual aspects.6,7 Photic interacts with non-photic at the retinal level rather than being completely independent,8 which determines the combined visual, affective, and physiological effects.4,9 In contrast to visual effects, non-visual effects focus on circadian rhythms and neural circuits related to behavioral responses.10

The physical properties of lighting generally comprise illuminance or brightness,11,12 correlated color temperature (CCT),12 and spectral power distribution (including light wavelength).13,14 Illuminance meets visible requirements or maintains alertness.15 Meanwhile, the spectral profile is composed of various light wavelengths, where the color temperature divides the visible light into warm and cool type.16 Furthermore, the circadian stimulus (CS), the melanopic equivalent daylight illuminance (mEDI), and the equivalent melanopic lux (EML) are leading models for measuring rhythmic effects. Nevertheless, the CIE S 026/E:2018 considers the light-sensitive contribution of ipRGCs toward non-visual effects alone,17 while the WELL building standard proposes an EML-based model for all spaces. CS metrics were obtained as a function of amounted circadian lux (CLA) by Rea et al,18 and artificial light with CS values above 0.3 facilitated rhythmic regulation.19 Although common models depend on spectral sensitivity quantities, the use of general metrics has emerged as the circadian index inferred from illuminance, CCT, and color rendering index (CRI).20 Notably, the consistency of judging the CRI21 is unstable owing to the diversity of luminaire types. Hence, illuminance, CCT, and mEDI are key features of a lighting environment. To date, evidence from existing studies suggests a relationship between lighting parameters and non-visual effects of light exposures. In a practical scenario of submarine patrol with 24-h-based lighting schedules, Young et al compared sleep efficiency (SE), sleepiness, melatonin concentration, and individual performance of high CCT (13500 K) and standard-issue CCT (4100 K).22 For the high CCT, its sleepiness score was lower, but the CS value was higher. Similarly, for a traditional fluorescent lamp (high CCT, 6000 K) and a light-emitting diode (low CCT, 2000 K) in a bedroom setting, Wen et al measured sleep quantity and quality, drowsiness, and fatigue in the next morning.23 The low CCT significantly reduced next-morning sleepiness. Three night light settings (40 lx at 6500, 2500, and 3000 K) were studied, in which 6500 K resulted in greater melatonin suppression and increased alertness.24 The high CCT is more conducive to maintaining work schedules and human behavioral alignment, whereas subjects exposed to low CCT may have better sleep quality and fatigue recovery. However, CCT and illuminance are often inseparable. Kruithof pioneered the two to form a comfort dimension of illuminance as the vertical axis and color temperature as the horizontal.25 Experiments on three consecutive nights revealed the bright light effects of early evening (1200 lx, 4000 K) vs later evening (750 lx, 4000 K) on melatonin levels, subjective sleepiness, body temperature, and skin blood flow.26 Ru et al studied the diurnal effects of illuminance (100 vs 1000 lx) and CCT (3000 vs 6500 K) on subjective mood and cognitive evaluation.12 The effects of indoor illuminance (1200 vs 200 lx) and CCT (3000 vs 6500 K) on daytime cognitive performance, subjective mood, and alertness were examined in healthy adults.27 Based on the above-mentioned studies, both CCT and illuminance settings need to be further investigated in specific sleep-related scenarios.28 Previous studies have not provided detailed variations for sleep parameters in the context of CCT and illuminance.

The time-space distribution of diverse light sources is another crucial consideration that determines the spatial distribution of light, dynamic patterns, and duration of exposure. Particularly, blue-depleted evening ambient light reduced melatonin suppression and alertness.29 Likewise, amber-lens glasses are recommended to block short-wave light exposure, or reduce prebedtime media use to enhance sleep quality of recreational athletes.30 Yet, fewer studies have examined the effects of time-dependent light factors (eg exposure time or duration)31 on sleep.32 Three light conditions, namely alternating intermittent blue-enriched bright lights (~1000 lx, ~6000 K) and normal dark light (~5 lx, ~3600 K), continuous bright light, and continuous dark light, were tested for subjective alertness (SA), and subsequent sleep structure after a prebedtime 3-hour (3-h) exposure.33 Compared to dark light, intermittent and continuous bright light significantly increase SA but lower SE and total sleep time (TST).

Sleep is a ubiquitous biological phenomenon. Models of sleep regulation are the basis for exploring the intrinsic physiological mechanisms of sleep-wakefulness; for example, the two-process model involves sleep-dependent homeostatic processes (Process S) and sleep-independent circadian rhythmic processes (Process C).34,35 The interaction between the two processes determines sleep propensity and sleep duration. The circadian system adapts to the time of day by sensing outside light signals and adjusting an organism’s routine in the real-world. Phenotypic indicators of sleep rhythms have been explored as tools for assessing the effect of actual lights;36,37 for example, students in high CCT settings had significantly later bedtime and sleep onset time than those in low CCT.38

The targeted enclosed space provides artificial lighting and a controllable Heating, Ventilation and Air Conditioning (HVAC) system, the situation of which is similar to that of ships or submarine vessels.39 An enclosed space, as a place with restricted access, allows people to work or sleep within a period of time.40,41 In an actual scenario, naval crews work in artificially lit areas without exposure to sunlight.28 For the enclosed space onboard a submarine,42 the compact work/rest schedule implies almost no resting gap from work to rest and back to work. Additionally, the submariners encountered a non-entrained sleep-wake pattern under a tight and atypical work scheduling arrangement, but they were still obliged to continue the voyage.43,44 Hence, in such a situation, the crew’s psychophysiological performance undergoes subtle changes, some of which incorporate sleep, alertness, and affect.43 The references2,45,46 are dedicated to influential research on light; however, few scholars have explored whether prolonged pre-bedtime exposure to bright light at work and forced resting dark environment in enclosed spaces have an impact on seafarers’ performance. Moreover, prebedtime light exposure interventions (LEIs) mean changing lighting parameters, and thus impose a disruption; for example, irregular lightings lead to problems with circadian rhythms and sleep, which cause mood or learning deficits.47 Recently, irregular light/dark cycles could directly affect neuronal coordination by producing major disruptions in circadian activity and motor function.48 In addition, emotional state progressively affects subjective sleep quality.14

Inspired by established research, the present study aimed to investigate influential associations between sleep performance and human states according to the time-dependent effects of altered prebedtime light exposure on sleep performance and human states (corresponding to SA and negative affect (NA)) among young healthy adults. It included the following two concerns: (1) The effects of altered prebedtime light exposure from baseline to different interventions were studied under an enclosed space experimental paradigm. Statistical methods were employed to analyze the primary effect of the LEI groups. (2) For all dependent variables, process modelling was utilized to derive possible interrelated influencing relationships based on statistical effects. Systemic regulations or predictions could be implemented in accordance with the relationships.

Materials and Methods

Experimental Setup

A between-subjects factor (LEI: the illuminances at eye level are combined with CCTs based on the orthogonal experimental design) and one within-subjects factor (Time: baseline at D8 and intervention at D9, D10) are designated in Table 1. Moderate normal lighting was set as 300 lx and 4000 K referencing the ship standard.49 Intervention design considerations arise from enhancing or lowering light parameters, thereby the selection of which is based on CIE S 008/E-2001 recommended scales of illuminance, color appearance, and GB 50034–2013 building compartment limits.50,51 The uniformity of illuminance measured on the tabletop was 0.80 to 0.95, and general CRI ranges from 85 to 90. The land-based paradigm of enclosed space experiments provides available simulated lighting. Figure 1 depicts the spectrum power distribution profile, primarily concerned with the sensitivity to different light wavelengths because of the electromagnetic nature.52

Table 1 Description of LEI and Time Setting

Figure 1 Spectrum power distribution measured at eye level for one baseline (300 lx, 4000 K) and four light exposure intervention settings.

Physical length*width*height dimensions of the simulated workspace associated with light exposures, are 3.5*2.5*2.1 m in Figure 2, while the sizes of another different enclosed space, the sleep chamber, are 3.5*3.5*2.1 m. Three sleep chambers and one workspace are offered and connected via sliding doors and a corridor. The suitable temperature and humidity in the sleep chamber in Figure 2a were maintained at approximately 25 ± 2 °C and 50%. The spatial noise was controlled to maintain a quiet environment. The ambient illuminance of the sleeping experiment required no light or was less than 1 lx. The experimenters provided disposable sheets and pillowcases for sanitation. To control the combined LEI changes, a stable experimental scenario was required to implement the easy switching of the desired parameters, which consisted of a manual control area equipped with a Lenovo desktop computer in Figure 2c and an experimental area in Figure 2b with six ceiling-mounted luminaires (HJMN-ZK02 LED round lamps) manufactured by Beijing Yisheng Taihe Technology Company.

Figure 2 Experimental setting and scenes display via a 2-D graph of the whole experimental layout. (a) is a simulated sleep chamber equipped with a single bed, “3 × Sleep chamber” means that three identical sleep chambers are offered to 3 participants simultaneously; (b) is a simulated workspace with three seats and six LED round lamps in an enclosed space, where reading tasks are performed; (c) is a control area with a computer.

Notes: Researchers adjust light parameters of workspace from control area, and then transform participants from the workspace to the sleep chamber.

Participants

Based on the software toolkit G*Power 3.1.9.7,53 a repeated-measures, within-between interaction model was hypothesized prior to the formal experiment. To achieve 90% power, we settled on a correlation of 0.5 among repeated measurements, with a non-spherical correction epsilon of 1. The effect size of Cohen’s f value was 0.25, indicating moderate intervention effects. Input parameters and calculation results are shown in Table 2. Hence, a total sample size of at least 44 participants was required.

Table 2 Calculated Determination of Sample Size

Figure 3 shows a flowchart for selection of eligible participants.54 The participants completed the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) questionnaire in Chinese version30 for the past month before the experiment. Those who met the PSQI < 5 were included. Seventeen participants responding to extreme chronotypes based on the Chinese version of Horne and Ostberg’s Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (intermediate MEQ, 50–62) were excluded.55 The selected 59 postgraduates were eligible for the seven days’ dormitory sleep observation survey (from D1 to D7, D1–7), who were required to eat normally. Considering whether they had engaged in pre-experiment strenuous physical exertion activities, ingested stimulant substances, sleep aids, or painkillers within one week, and smoked, consumed alcohol, or had caffeinated beverages per day, 51 participants were recruited for the in-lab sleep experiment. All of them informed about the purpose of this study signed the written consent voluntarily engaging in the trials. Finally, 2 males and 1 female were excluded (D8–10) due to unfinished experiments, missing sleep data, and menstrual reasons. And thus, forty-eight postgraduates (male = 24) aged 23.58 ± 0.70 years were available for data analysis in total. This study was approved by the Institutional Ethical Committee of Northwest Polytechnical University (Grant No. 202202053) and was in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Figure 3 Flowchart of information on enrolled participants.

Abbreviations: PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; MEQ, Horne and Ostberg’s Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire.

Study Measurements

To maintain a natural work/sleep pattern unobtrusively as much as possible, some objective methods were not adopted; for example, polysomnography (PSG, including electroencephalography (EEG)) is a gold standard for recording multiple human parameters,56,57 but its electrodes are strongly bound for natural work/sleep activities. Several measurements performed to quantify the responsive outcomes are as follows.

Alertness Assessment via Subjective Rating

Sleepiness and alertness are the most frequent factors determining mental state. The Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) was suitable for measuring changes in the sleepiness or alertness of participants.30,58 Score ratings on the KSS range from 1 (extremely alert) to 9 (extremely sleepy, fighting sleep).

NA Assessment via Emotional Model

Well-known affect assessment models contain the Self-Assessment Manikin,59 Plutchik wheel model,60 Pleasure-Arousal-Dominance (PAD) affective model,61 and Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS).62 The Self-Assessment Manikin and the PAD affective model are very similar, and both are biased towards estimating levels of emotional arousal. The Plutchik wheel model comprises diverse affective adjectives; however, it is difficult to quantify the effect in a synthesized manner. In contrast, the PANAS scale is divided into two parts with emotional descriptors for positive affect and NA. Positive affect assessment consisted of interested, excited, strong, enthusiastic, proud, alert, inspired, determined, attentive, and active, whereas NA assessment covered distressed, upset, guilty, scared, hostile, irritable, ashamed, nervous, jittery, and afraid. Participants’ feelings, reactions, and levels of identification were measured using a five-point scale (1 – Almost not at all, 5 – Extremely much). Thus, the scores for all negative adjectives were aggregated into NA outcomes.

Sleep Recording via Actigraphy, Sleep Diary, and Self-Reported Sleep Survey

Throughout the in-lab trial, we performed wrist actigraphy-based natural sleep measures along with feedbacks from the participants. An actigraphy device depends on the acceleration sensor-based vector amplitudes synthesized from three-axis (x, y, z) motion data. A wrist actigraphy (wGT3X-BT, United States) is usually placed on the limb part with the least impact on the body movement range, eg the non-dominant left wrist. Actigraphy-based sleep monitoring continuously obtains subjects’ movement characteristics, thereby yielding the sleep onset latency (SOL), TST, wake after sleep onset (WASO), SE and sleep fragmentation index (SFI) with the Cole-Kripke algorithm. Sleep diary and actigraphy-based measures are complementary. Sleep monitoring provides a numerical basis, yet it is typically inadequate. In-depth insight into the complex sleep-wake profile was gained from the recorded sleep diary. To learn participants’ epistemic uncertainties, sufficiency and quality of sleep were directly investigated via the Next-day Self-assessment Sleep Quality (NSSQ) questionnaire63,64 in Table 3. “Do you think you get enough sleep?” denotes the perception of sleep adequacy, which reflects participants’ sleep sensations. “Overall sleep quality?” focuses on the sleep quality ratings.

Table 3 Next-Day Self-Assessment Sleep Quality Questionnaire

Study Procedures

The study procedures included two phases; Phase 1 – in dormitory (D1–7) and phase 2 – in laboratory (D8–10) are as shown in Figure 4. Besides being informed of demographic characteristics, seven days’ sleep-related survey and observation was conducted in a familiar dormitory, where room temperature was maintained at 25 ± 2 °C. We learned about the routine habits of these participants from a sleep diary and details of the PSQI questionnaire, which also contained daily sleep hygiene and mealtime. Hence, the prebedtime 3-h33 light exposure was specified for phase 2, taking the nightly timing of falling asleep inferred from the sleep diary and adequate metabolic time reserved after dinner65 as a reference. The experimental LEI occurred approximately 19:30 h to 23:30 h, and the adjustable timing was flexibly controlled within 30 minutes. Meanwhile, combined with habitual sleep bedtime or sleep onset, the experimental sleep duration was identified as eight hours accordingly. To reduce the first-night effect, LEIs started with one baseline night (D8) in an enclosed space, followed by two consecutive intervention nights’ trials (D9 and D10). Three participants were allowed to conduct experiments in parallel. The study included 51 laboratory nights trials and 413 dormitory nights observations, and lasted for approximately two months in winter.

Figure 4 Study procedure and protocol design.

Abbreviations: LEI, light exposure intervention; KSS, Karolinska Sleepiness Scale; PANAS, Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; NSSQ, Next-day Self-assessment Sleep Quality.

Notes: the whole experiment was categorized into two phases, including a survey and observation in dormitory (D1–7) and light exposure trials in laboratory (D8–10). Eight-hour sleep is required in laboratory. Particularly, four points of measurement (t1–t4) are assigned at proper times for phase 2. 0 h means the end of light exposure.

For phase 2, 30 minutes of participants’ preparation (eg bathing) were reserved for the LEI trial in addition to preparing the laboratory in advance. To measure psychophysiological changes, discrete measurement points were configured as the start of timing (t1), before going to bed (t2), when waking up (t3), and 1-h after waking up (t4). For the four-measurement time, we gauged alertness via the KSS and NA via the PANAS, and figured out the self-reported sleep survey via the NSSQ questionnaire after waking up per day. Participants wore wrist actigraphy throughout the experiment. The effects of light emitted by electronic devices are imposed on alertness, sleep, and affect in the references.66,67 To avoid the influence of blue light from electronic devices from t1 to t2 as much as possible,14 we provided three alternative paper reading materials (eg specialty books, science fiction stories, and English test papers), which were randomly assigned to one of three days. The mobile phone carried was placed in a shielded box from t1 to t4 and was permitted to check its messages once or twice before t2, but restricted to one minute. Participants had no access to the mobile phone from t2 to t4. The cumulative duration of using electronic devices during the day was limited to two hours. Additionally, the participants maintained their most comfortable sitting posture, and three or seven short breaks were allowed for trials. The primary work of these postgraduate subjects during daytime hours was reading and attending classes. Some preferred the usage of eye masks because of self-habits from t2 to t3, although they were in a dark–light sleep chamber. In the morning, they could choose to wake up naturally or with an alarm clock. Specifically, they were asked to complete the NSSQ questionnaire with a sleep diary from t3 to t4 and allowed to leave the sleep chamber after confirming the observation.

Data Analysis

Firstly, singular values were examined and eliminated by exceeding the interquartile range (IQR) by a factor of 1.5. Only two NA values at t1 and t2 of D9 were removed. Considering time-dependent factors is reflected in the delta values of prebedtime (t1 and t2) and awakening (t3 and t4) periods. For the SA and NA, the delta value (∆P) calculation of variations at prebedtime and awakening is given by Equation (1), respectively.

(1)

An overview of the methodology, process of data processing, and analysis is presented in Figure 5. Raw data were measured and collected from the perspective of human state and sleep performance, and preprocessed to form the updated datasets. Due to repeated measurements, a general linear model (GLM) was applied to determine the main effect of LEI and Time, and the interaction effect between LEI and Time using SPSS Statistical Software 27.0. Descriptive statistics were demonstrated by the estimated marginal means (EMM) and standard errors of the mean (SEM) of four groups. The level of significance was set as 0.05. Discrepancies between pairwise comparison groups were further characterized, and we completed a post hoc test of pairwise groups (#L1–#L4 in Table 1) or pairwise Times by Fisher least significant difference (LSD) Test method.

Figure 5 Methodology and process of data processing and analysis.

The Pearson correlation coefficient is credited as a relational analysis of dependent variables ranging from −1 (perfect negative correlation) to +1 (perfect positive correlation). User experience and activity theory categorizes sleep within the built environment into a variety of manifestations: before, during, and after sleep.68,69 To mitigate the influence of irrelevant variables, the maximum variance-based exploratory factor analysis (EFA) is superior in reflecting the measured variables before bedtime, during sleep, and after waking up. Additionally, path analysis was estimated with process model to uncover potential relationships, whereas a bootstrapping algorithm was applied owing to the small sample size. Finally, Smart-PLS 4.0 software suitable for generating the estimated path was chosen. Modeling of the relational effects of sleep-related stages contributed to data analysis for path coefficients and significance levels (p < 0.05).

Results

As its reliability test satisfied Spherical Symmetry (Mauchly’s W = 0.973, p = 0.768) in the example of the change in SA before bedtime (SA_Prebedtime), a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was first performed. Contrastingly, perceived sleep adequacy (PSA) dissatisfied the test of Sphericity Symmetry (Mauchly’s W = 0.631, p < 0.05); hence, the results of Greenhouse-Geisser’s Test were adopted. The results of statistical effects are shown in Table 4. Levene’s homogeneity test indicated that the probability greater than 0.05 was in accordance with a normal distribution. Almost no interaction effect existed between the LEI and Time. Notably, the interaction effect (p = 0.058) between the LEI and the Time on the change in NA before bedtime (NA_Prebedtime) was marginally significant. The main effect of the Time was significantly different in SA_Prebedtime, PSA, and perceived sleep quality (PSQ), while the main effect of the LEI was merely in sleep onset latency (SOL).

Table 4 Results for Statistical Effects of Dependent Variables

Effect of Light Exposure on Subjective Alertness

An SA score of five (= “neither alert nor sleepy”) is a key cutoff point for alertness or sleepiness of the human state;58 for example, the SA at t1 reached above 5 in Figure 6. Among these LEI groups, the subjects did not hit high levels of alertness soon after waking up (t3), which was always lower than before bedtime (t2). The recovery of alertness in high illuminance and high CCT (#L1) after waking up is the slowest in Figure 6b. Yet this study emphasized the relative changes in alertness. Hence, Figure 7a and b displayed the alertness variation before and after sleep under the four groupings, respectively. When exposed to continuous lighting, human alertness decreased while the sleepiness increased during the prebedtime 3-h period. The insignificant interaction effect between the LEI and the Time leads to a Time-focused impact analysis; for instance, the intervention and the baseline on the Time were significantly different as depicted in Table 5 and the probability of significance (p < 0.05) is marked in Figure 7. Low warm light (#L4) had the most evident increase in sleepiness at prebedtime, because the mean difference of D8 minus D9 was −1.83 (significant, p = 0.009), or the mean difference of D8 minus D10 was −1.67 (significant, p = 0.035). The overall EMM of SA_Prebedtime is 1.94 (SEM = 0.20). In detail, both #L2 and #L4 had larger SA_Prebedtime estimates than the overall EMM, suggesting that changing to low illuminance conditions was more likely to cause sleepiness. Although the SA_Prebedtime estimates for #L1 and #L3 were lower than the overall EMM, the mean sleepiness increased over time when exposed to prebedtime light. The estimated alertness at awakening for the remaining three lighting conditions exceed the overall EMM level. Combined with the statistical effect, altering the lighting condition had no significant effect on alertness variation at awakening but was significant at t1 and t2 of the prebedtime.

Table 5 Mean Difference and Significance of the Changes in Subjective Alertness Before Bedtime Between Different Times

Figure 6 Mean of subjective alertness for (a) t1 and t2 at prebedtime, (b) t3 and t4 at awakening.

Figure 7 Estimated marginal means of the changes in subjective alertness: (a) before bedtime, (b) after waking up, and the changes in negative affect: (c) before bedtime, (d) after waking up. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. #L1 - #L4 indicate groupings from baseline to intervention distinguished by LEI. Significant is marked, *p < 0.05.

Effect of Light Exposure on Negative Affect

Figure 7c and d illustrated the results of NA_Prebedtime and change in NA after waking up (NA_Awakening). The NA_Prebedtime of participants on three days (D8–10) was negative. At D8, NA_Prebedtime between #L1 and #L4 was significant (its mean difference of #L1 minus #L4 was −5.33, p = 0.020). However, mean of the NA was very high at the t1 timing of D8 in Figure 8a. At D9, #L2 differed from #L1 (the mean NA_Prebedtime difference of #L2 minus #L1 was −2.33, p = 0.054) and #L3 (the mean NA_Prebedtime difference of #L2 minus #L3 was −2.00, p = 0.094), respectively, where a marginally significant effect was observed. When changing to high illuminance and high CCT (#L1), a significant difference between D8 and D9 (p = 0.006) suggested that the enhancement of lighting parameters had a positive effect on NA regardless of prebedtime or awakening in Table 6. The NA varied less significantly among measured outcomes after waking up. The rhythm of NA variations in the uplifted illuminance groups (#L1 and #L3) was highly synchronized. After waking up, a boundedly significant discrepancy between #L1 and #L4 at D9 (the mean NA_Awakening difference of #L1 minus #L4 was −2.83, p = 0.071). Especially for #L1, there was an obvious difference between D8 and D9 after waking up (see Table 6). Compared to the numerical variations of NA delta values at prebedtime, the outcome of NA_Awakening was chaotic, which may be attributed to the neuroregulatory impact of sleep recovery70 or other causes to be explored.

Table 6 Mean Difference and Significance of the Changes in Negative Affect Before Bedtime and After Waking Up for #L1 Between Different Times

Figure 8 Mean of negative affect for (a) t1 and t2 at prebedtime, (b) t3 and t4 at awakening.

Effect of Light Exposure on Sleep Performance

The EMM profiles of subjective sleep are described in Figure 9. For PSA and PSQ, no participants experienced very bad sleep quality. The global EMM of PSA was 0.708 (SEM = 0.072), whereas that of PSQ was 2.847 (SEM = 0.087) indicating that all LEI groups exceeded its median level. A remarkable difference between the intervention of D10 and the baseline of D8 was described in Table 7. Additionally, a marginal significance was found between the PSQ of #L1 and #L3 at D10 (the mean PSQ difference of #L1 minus #L3 was 0.67, p = 0.059). The number of participants with insufficient sleep and poor sleep quality on D8 was higher than that on D9–10. Subjects with high illuminance and low CCT (#L3) were more likely to suffer from insufficient sleep. However, participants who underwent high illuminance intervention (#L1 and #L3) were less likely to have poor sleep quality.

Table 7 Mean Difference and Significance of Perceived Sleep Adequacy and Quality Between Different Times

Figure 9 Estimated marginal means of (a) PSA, perceived sleep adequacy, and (b) PSQ, perceived sleep quality. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. #L1 - #L4 indicate groupings from baseline to intervention distinguished by LEI. Significant is marked, * p < 0.05.

The five sleep parameter estimations under the four LEI groups are presented in Figure 10. When switching to low CCT lighting, the mean difference in TST between #L3 and #L4 showed that high illuminance at prebedtime significantly shortened sleep duration compared to low illuminance; specifically, the mean TST difference of #L3 minus #L4 was −0.70 hours (p = 0.019) at D8, while that of #L3 minus #L4 was −0.47 hours (p = 0.048) at D9. The mean TST difference of #L2 minus #L4 was significantly distinct (−0.47 hours, p = 0.047) at D9. For #L3, the TST outcome of post-intervention was distinguished from the baseline (the mean TST difference of D8 minus D9 was −0.37 hours, p = 0.036). As such, this result reminds us of the ability to acutely lower the color temperature and increase the illuminance (#L3) at prebedtime for enhanced sleep duration.

Figure 10 Estimated marginal means of sleep parameters: (a) TST, total sleep time; (b) SOL, sleep onset latency; (c) WASO, wake after sleep onset; (d) SE, sleep efficiency; and (e) SFI, sleep fragmentation index. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. #L1 - #L4 indicate groupings from baseline to intervention distinguished by LEI. Significant is marked, *p < 0.05.

Figure 10b depicts the results of the SOL under different LEI groups. The SOL reveals whether the participants fall asleep normally. The SOL time for #L2 was longer than that for the other three groups, indicating more difficulty falling asleep. However, from D8 to D10, the SOLs for #L2 were very higher. Particularly, #L2 significantly differentiated from #L1 (the mean SOL difference of #L2 minus #L1 was 9.39 minutes, p = 0.007) and #L4 (the mean SOL difference of #L2 minus #L4 was −8.33 minutes, p = 0.014) in Table 8. #L1 was marginally different from #L2 (the mean SOL difference of #L1 minus #L2 was −7.33 minutes, p = 0.058) at D9.

Table 8 Mean Difference and Significance of Sleep Onset Latency (Minutes) Between Different LEIs

The WASO results are exhibited in Figure 10c. Compared to #L3, both #L1 (the mean WASO difference of #L1 minus #L3 was −28.17 minutes, p = 0.045) and #L4 (the mean WASO difference of #L3 minus #L4 was 38.00 minutes, p = 0.009) had a shorter awakening time after sleep onset at D8. Besides, as compared to #L4, #L1 (the mean WASO difference of #L1 minus #L4 was 28.00 minutes, p = 0.051) and #L3 (the mean WASO difference of #L3 minus #L4 was 27.67 minutes, p = 0.053) had longer awakening time after sleep onset at D9–10. Furthermore, regarding the SE in Figure 10d, the SE results exceed the normal threshold of 80%. #L3 significantly differentiated from #L1 (the mean SE difference of #L3 minus #L1 was −7.34%, p = 0.031) and #L4 (the mean SE difference of #L3 minus #L4 was −8.50%, p = 0.014) at D8, while #L2 (the mean SE difference of #L2 minus #L4 was −6.26%, p = 0.046) and #L3 (the mean SE difference of #L3 minus #L4 was −6.28%, p = 0.045) manifested lower SE values than #L4 significantly at D9. Lastly, the SFI variables did not show statistically significant at D9–10 in Figure 10e. Although the assessment of sleep disturbance theoretically depended on the SFI, its ultimate statistical result was insignificant.

Sleep Performance in Relation to Human States

The paired correlation coefficients are used to characterize the relationships between all metrics in Table 9. The SA_Prebedtime was associated with PSA variable (r = 0.260, p = 0.028). The NA_Awakening correlated with the SA_Awakening (r = 0.247, p = 0.037). Notably, the PSQ was strongly correlated with the PSA (r = 0.633, p < 0.01). Sleep parameters were interrelated except for an insignificant correlation between SOL and SFI; for example, the TST was negatively correlated with the SOL (r = −0.450, p < 0.01) and the WASO (r = −0.946, p < 0.01).

Table 9 Correlation Coefficient Calculation Results Among Different Variables

For all datasets, four types of factors were obtained from the EFA method in Table 10. In most studies, ± 0.4 is adopted as the cutoff value for factor loadings.71 Regarding the outcomes during different stages, it is thereby named as “Factor 1 - objective sleep”, “Factor 2 - subjective sleep”, “Factor 3 - awakening state”, and “Factor 4 - prebedtime state”, respectively. Herein, human states correspond to the prebedtime state and the awakening state. The four factors accounted for 72.4% of the cumulative variance contribution ratio as indicated by the calculated total variance. Each factor had an initial eigenvalue of greater than 1.0. To our knowledge, although subjective and objective sleep are correlated theoretically, the complexity of sleep phenomenon makes sleep parameters less interpretable than the self-reported sleep. SOL is assigned not only to Factor 1 but also to Factor 4, which indicates the fact that the SOL is an objective sleep parameter and infers a connection between SOL and the prebedtime state as well.

Table 10 Rotated Factor Loadings of All Metrics

The path coefficient is one of standardized regression coefficients that indicates the direct effect of one variable on another. According to the estimated path analysis in Figure 11, the prebedtime state and SOL were negatively correlated (t = 2.075, p = 0.038) demonstrating that lowered alertness and reduced NA at prebedtime caused by light intervention are beneficial for a person to fall asleep as soon as possible. The SOL positively correlated with objective sleep assessments (t = 2.740, p = 0.006). Consequently, the SOL determines the level of objective sleep quality in the context of altered prebedtime light exposures. However, no immediate significant correlation was found between prebedtime state and either objective or subjective sleep (see Figure 11). The regression coefficient highlighted the statistical significance between subjective sleep and awakening state (t = 2.508, p = 0.012). A better subjective sleep rating corresponds to a better wakefulness state. Additionally, subjective sleep outcomes were close to correlate with objective sleep (t = 1.750, p = 0.080). Hence, in the course of participant interaction with lighting ergonomics factors, it developed a chained pathway relationship of “prebedtime state - SOL - objective sleep - subjective sleep - awakening state” based on the aforementioned datasets.

Figure 11 Results for standardized path analysis with a process model. Rectangles denote the latent variable, of which directed lines represent the value of path coefficients (p value).

Abbreviation: SOL, sleep onset latency.

Notes: Solid directed lines indicate statistical significance (p < 0.05), while dashed directed lines represent relationships that are not statistically significant.

Discussion

This study explored the time-dependent effects of altered prebedtime light exposure in enclosed spaces on sleep performance associated with human states using statistical analysis and process modelling. Our findings suggest that objective sleep parameters and subjective sleep assessments during sleep are associated with the SA and NA at prebedtime and awakening among young healthy adults.

In the current study, the LEI and the Time were considered as independent variables. For the experimental setup, illuminance and CCT as a fixed LEI factor provide four varying conditions. Compared with other studies, we set the illuminance in the medium threshold which is an intermediate between the higher (eg 5000 lx)45 and low (eg 40 lx)24 light intensity. Exposure to low illuminance and high color temperature (40 lx, 6500 K) in a previous study24 resulted in enhanced SAs. The SAs of low illuminance and high CCT (#L2) at t1 and t2 were also observed to increase from baseline in Figure 6. The effect of 106 lx of illuminance for 4-h continuous exposure had little change on the SA (maintained KSS = 3), until 5.5 h when the KSS increased to 2 by Cajochen et al.15 After 3-h light exposure, both #L2 and #L4 groups elevated the SA more than the other two, with the SA approaching 3 at the t2 timing of intervention, suggesting that the KSS-assessed SA outcomes were fairly reliable.

Different office light CCTs had significant main effects on SA, and the average KSS of 4000 K (= 5.41) was significantly higher than the other three higher CCTs, but the longer light exposure duration was only 90min.72 In contrast, the baseline condition with 3-h light exposure in this study resulted in an inconsistent average KSS (= 6.33) at t2, which was higher than 5.41. In addition, various color temperatures in the office (4000, 6000, 8000, and 10,000 K at 300lx) had no significant effect on determined, annoyed, and anxious, while there was a significant main effect on happy, excited, inspired.72 In our study, although no significant effect of the LEI on the NA was also observed, receiving LEIs from t1 to t2 caused the NA scores to decrease, which inferred that continuous exposure to bright light could alleviate NAs at prebedtime. Due to different ratios of male and female in Figure 3, a higher mean NA at the t1 of D8 was observed and some NA values of D9 has been excluded from the #L1 group. The result may be explained by the fact that these subjects require adaptation to enclosed spaces at the same time, and partly due to random assignment. In the case of the gender imbalance, higher NA values for an individual subject would likely result in larger mean of the calculated NA. Altered prebedtime light exposures might interfere with psycho-affective responses at awakening while ensuring adequate sleep (eg the mean PSQ was perceived as “fairly good” and the SE met over 80%).

The 3-h light exposure duration makes this study different from other studies, such as 1.5-h,72,73 1-h,17,23 and 0.5-h74 light exposure. These existing studies did not consider the time as a factor, but instead focused on spectral power-related light illuminance or CCT. After a 3-h continuous prebedtime light exposure, the main effect of the Time on the SA_Prebedtime and subjective sleep (refer to PSA and PSQ) was significant, which guided us to think about time-dependent relationships between human states and sleep performance. Additionally, an experimental study examined the diurnal effects of office lighting on alertness, mood, and cognitive performance,12 but the time was set to daytime hours (10:00 h to 17:00 h). The experimental period of this study was approximately from 19:30 h to 9:00 h, and the human responses were differently mirrored on the timeline.

The calculated delta changes in human states (refer to SA and NA) from t1 to t2 (or from t3 to t4) are the relative values mapping psychophysiological responses. Subtle human state changes caused by shifts in the medium-intensity light exposure were observed at prebedtime (3-h) and awakening (1-h). During 3 consecutive laboratory days, we found that the measured changes in the SA at D9–10 nights were significantly different from that at D8 night (see Table 5). Figure 6a depicts that with continuous bright-light exposure overall alertness is keeping a trend of rising. The time-dependent significant effect of light exposure on SA changes at prebedtime disappeared upon awakening, but exerted a correlative impact on the SOL (see Figure 11). Furthermore, in the case of switching to the high illuminance and high CCT (#L1), the change in NA at prebedtime of D9 was obviously higher than that at prebedtime of D8, but the change in NA at awakening of D9 was lower than that at awakening of D8 within the same group. After experiencing 8-h sleep, NAs of participants in the #L1 group were observed to be disturbed at D9, yet the other three groups were less likely to be disturbed.

Through an EFA method, it was validated that subjective sleep consists of the PSA and the PSQ. For the PSA and PSQ, a significant increase of assessments was observed from D8 to D10. However, no such differences were found in the five estimated sleep parameters. The significant main effect of the LEI on the SOL was found and insignificant interaction effect between the LEI and time on sleep parameters was observed. Hence, we reasoned that as the time spent in the laboratory accumulated, the participants psychologically adapted to interacting with the LEIs and the enclosed space sleep environment.

Assessing sleep performance relies on subjective and objective sleep outcomes. Firstly, although the joint assessment of wrist actigraphy and sleep diaries has been validated by many previous studies (including a comparison between actigraphy and PSG),75,76 baseline differences of sleep parameters between groups at D8 were observed in this study. This outcome may be rooted in some factors including: (1) the implicit presence of individual differences under the four groupings; (2) the psychological adaptation to the enclosed space environment; (3) the explicit existence of data differences in the proportion of males among subgroups (see Figure 3). The three factors pointed more or less to the limitations of the experimental protocol. It was worth mentioning that these estimated sleep parameters at D8–10 were evenly compared, but the observed differences between the baseline and the intervention were partially significant. The SOL served as an essential outcome variable. In addition, the lower correlated color temperature with higher illuminance nocturnal light environment helped to improve sleep quality,77 yet we did not find similar effects of high illuminance and low CCT (#L3) on the sleep parameters. The reason for this might be due to the difference in spectral energy emitted by LEDs. Second, strong correlations were found between five estimated objective sleep parameters. Meanwhile, an EFA revealed that the subjective and objective sleep were not be equivalent. Therefore, combining subjective and objective sleep outcome evidence is necessary.

To our knowledge, this is the first study to explore the association between sleep performance and human states at different stages from a holistic viewpoint in the contextual LEI environment. The sleep phenomenon is a form of periodic restorative activity that is difficult to decipher.35 Importantly, the process modelling applied in this study helped to construct time-dependent relationships. Sleep performance and human states constituted a coherent and predictive pathway. Quantitative path regression coefficients provided a predictive basis for accurately judging individual sleep performance associated with human states at prebedtime or awakening. One key finding in this study was that the SOL became a mediating predictor for the prebedtime state and objective sleep. Moreover, subjective sleep served as a predictor for the awakening state. A precise chained pathway can be iterated through further data collection and model upgrades.

There may be several limitations in this study. Although this experimental protocol did not allow for the use of cell phones or tablets, exposure to similar or shorter wavelength lighting outside the laboratory is difficult to fully control.67,78 The potential effects of age groups79 and gender80 were excluded. The sample size is modest (n=48), and thus a cohort-based big data collection81 will be considered in the future. Moreover, only a limited set of sleep-related variables was considered, and actigraphy-derived sleep parameters had serious limitations.76 When measuring individual sleep performances, other potential metrics are worth exploring, such as sleep architecture, sleep continuity, deep sleep, and slow-wave sleep. Importantly, the current study did not have accurate data on daytime light and work intensity, but no vigorous exercise was emphasized in the informed consent and 3 participants are excluded in Figure 3. Given previous research, the combination of subjective and objective measurements was the best option.79 This study was done in the laboratory, but the construction of time-dependent relationships still relied on the actual human performance in a high-fidelity field environment. The effects of LEI on participants were complicated, and seasonal effects might be omitted throughout the experiment. In a real-world maritime context, the external conditions (eg sea state) under which the actual ship is traveling may have an effect.

Conclusions

Our study reveals that sleep performance is associated with human states at prebedtime and awakening in the context of exploring the time-dependent effects of altered prebedtime light exposure in enclosed spaces on sleep performance, the SA, and the NA. The Time has a significant effect on subjective sleep and changes in prebedtime alertness. The SOL, as a critical sleep metric, plays a mediating predictor between prebedtime state and objective sleep, which is linked to the awakening state through subjective sleep.

Abbreviations

ANOVA, analysis of variance; CCT, correlated color temperature; CRI, color rendering index; CS, circadian stimulus; EEG, electroencephalography; EFA, exploratory factor analysis; EML, equivalent melanopic lux; EMM, estimated marginal means; GLM, general linear model; HVAC, Heating, Ventilation and Air Conditioning; ipRGCs, intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells; IQR, interquartile range; KSS, Karolinska Sleepiness Scale; LED, light-emitting diode; LEI, light exposure intervention; LSD, least significant difference; mEDI, melanopic equivalent daylight illuminance; MEQ, Ostberg’s Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire; NA, Negative affect; NA_Awakening, change in negative affect after waking up; NA_Prebedtime, change in negative affect before bedtime; NSSQ, Next-day Self-assessment Sleep Quality; PAD, Pleasure-Arousal-Dominance; PANAS, Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; PSA, Perceived sleep adequacy; PSG, polysomnography; PSQ, Perceived sleep quality; PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; SA, Subjective alertness; SA_Awakening, change in subjective alertness after waking up; SA_Prebedtime, change in subjective alertness before bedtime; SE, sleep efficiency; SEM, standard errors of the mean; SFI, sleep fragmentation index; SOL, sleep onset latency; TST, total sleep time; WASO, wake after sleep onset.

Funding

This study was supported by the Shaanxi Province Key R&D Program Project (2022GY-311) and the Shaanxi Province Special Support Program for Leading Talents (W099115).

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

1. Blume C, Garbazza C, Spitschan M. Effects of light on human circadian rhythms, sleep and mood. Somnologie. 2019;23:147–156. doi:10.1007/s11818-019-00215-x

2. Aulsebrook AE, Jones TM, Mulder RA, Lesku JA. Impacts of artificial light at night on sleep: a review and prospectus. J Exp Zool. 2018;329:409–418. doi:10.1002/jez.2189

3. Cain SW, McGlashan EM, Vidafar P, et al. Evening home lighting adversely impacts the circadian system and sleep. Sci Rep. 2020;10:1–10. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-75622-4

4. Bellia L, Bisegna F, Spada G. Lighting in indoor environments: visual and non-visual effects of light sources with different spectral power distributions. Build Environ. 2011;46(10):1984–1992. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2011.04.007

5. Berson DM, Dunn FA, Takao M. Phototransduction by retinal ganglion cells that set the circadian clock. Science. 2002;295(5557):1070–1073. doi:10.1126/science.1067262

6. Zhang Z, Beier C, Weil T, Hattar S. The retinal ipRGC-preoptic circuit mediates the acute effect of light on sleep. Nat Commun. 2021;12:1–11. doi:10.1038/s41467-021-25378-w

7. Fisk AS, Tam SKE, Brown LA, Vyazovskiy VV, Bannerman DM, Peirson SN. Light and cognition: roles for circadian rhythms, sleep, and arousal. Front Neurol. 2018;9:1–18. doi:10.3389/fneur.2018.00056

8. van Bommel W, van den Beld G. Lighting for work: a review of visual and biological effects. Lighting Res Technol. 2004;36(4):255–266. doi:10.1191/1365782804li122oa

9. Burns AC, Saxena R, Vetter C, Phillips AJK, Lane JM, Cain SW. Time spent in outdoor light is associated with mood, sleep, and circadian rhythm-related outcomes: a cross-sectional and longitudinal study in over 400,000 UK Biobank participants. J Affect Disord. 2021;295:347–352. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2021.08.056

10. Gou Z, Gou B, Liao W, Bao Y, Deng Y. Integrated lighting ergonomics: a review on the association between non-visual effects of light and ergonomics in the enclosed cabins. Build Environ. 2023;110616. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2023.110616

11. Prayag A, Münch M, Aeschbach D, Chellappa S, Gronfier C. Light modulation of human clocks, wake, and sleep. Clocks Sleep. 2019;1:193–208. doi:10.3390/clockssleep1010017

12. Ru T, de Kort YAW, Smolders KCHJ, Chen Q, Zhou G. Non-image forming effects of illuminance and correlated color temperature of office light on alertness, mood, and performance across cognitive domains. Build Environ. 2019;149:253–263. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2018.12.002

13. Figueiro MG, Sahin L, Roohan C, Kalsher M, Plitnick B, Rea MS. Effects of red light on sleep inertia. Nat Sci Sleep. 2019;11:45–57. doi:10.2147/NSS.S195563

14. Schmid SR, Höhn C, Bothe K, et al. How smart is it to go to bed with the phone? The impact of short-wavelength light and affective states on sleep and circadian rhythms. Clocks Sleep. 2021;3:558–580. doi:10.3390/clockssleep3040040

15. Cajochen C, Zeitzer JM, Czeisler CA, Dijk D-J. Dose-response relationship for light intensity and ocular and electroencephalographic correlates of human alertness. Behav Brain Res. 2000;115(1):75–83. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00236-9

16. Chen R, Tsai M-C, Tsay Y-S. Effect of color temperature and illuminance on psychology, physiology, and productivity: an experimental study. Energies. 2022;15(12):4477. doi:10.3390/en15124477

17. International Commission on lllumination. CIE system for metrology of optical radiation for iPRGC-influenced responses to light. CIE S 026/E. 32;2018. doi:10.25039/S026.2018

18. Rea M, Figueiro M. Light as a circadian stimulus for architectural lighting. Light Res Technol. 2018;50(4):497–510. doi:10.1177/1477153516682368

19. Figueiro M, Kalsher M, Steverson B, Heerwagen J, Kampschroer K, Rea M. Circadian-effective light and its impact on alertness in office workers. Light Res Technol. 2019;51(2):171–183. doi:10.1177/1477153517750006

20. Truong W, Zandi B, Trinh VQ, Khanh TQ. Circadian metric - Computation of circadian stimulus using illuminance, correlated colour temperature and colour rendering index.Build Environ. 2020;184:107146. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2020.107146

21. Liu X, Liu X, Jiang C, et al. A circadian lighting method for low blue light hazard, high color rendering index, and tunable circadian stimulus. Opt Laser Technol. 2024;168:109794. doi:10.1016/j.optlastec.2023.109794

22. Young CR, Jones GE, Figueiro MG, et al. At-sea trial of 24-h-based submarine watchstanding schedules with high and low correlated color temperature light sources. J Biol Rhythm. 2015;30(2):144–154. doi:10.1177/0748730415575432

23. Wen P, Tan F, Wu M, et al. The effects of different bedroom light environments in the evening on adolescents. Build Environ. 2021;206:108321. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2021.108321

24. Chellappa SL, Steiner R, Blattner P, Oelhafen P, Götz T, Cajochen C. Non-visual effects of light on melatonin, alertness and cognitive performance: can blue-enriched light keep us alert? PLoS One. 2011;6(1):e16429. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0016429

25. Noguchi H, Sakaguchi T. Effect of illuminance and color temperature on lowering of physiological activity. Appl Hum Sci. 1999;18(4):117–123. doi:10.2114/jpa.18.117

26. te Kulve M, Schlangen LJM, van Marken Lichtenbelt WD. Early evening light mitigates sleep compromising physiological and alerting responses to subsequent late evening light. Sci Rep. 2019;9:1–12. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-52352-w

27. Zhu Y, Yang M, Yao Y, et al. Effects of illuminance and correlated color temperature on daytime cognitive performance, subjective mood, and alertness in healthy adults. Environ Behav. 2019;51:199–230. doi:10.1177/0013916517738077

28. Shattuck NL, Matsangas P. Sunlight exposure, work hours, caffeine consumption, and sleep duration in the naval environment. Aerosp Med Hum Perf. 2017;88:579–585. doi:10.3357/AMHP.4721.2017

29. Rahman SA, St. Hilaire MA, Lockley SW. The effects of spectral tuning of evening ambient light on melatonin suppression, alertness and sleep. Physiol Behav. 2017;177:221–229. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.05.002

30. Knufinke M, Fittkau-Koch L, Møst EIS, Kompier MAJ, Nieuwenhuys A. Restricting short-wavelength light in the evening to improve sleep in recreational athletes–A pilot study. Eur J Sport Sci. 2019;19:728–735. doi:10.1080/17461391.2018.1544278

31. Wams EJ, Woelders T, Marring I, et al. Linking light exposure and subsequent sleep: a field polysomnography study in humans. Sleep. 2017;40(12):zsx165. doi:10.1093/sleep/zsx165

32. Dautovich ND, Schreiber DR, Imel JL, et al. A systematic review of the amount and timing of light in association with objective and subjective sleep outcomes in community-dwelling adults. Sleep Health. 2019;5:31–48. doi:10.1016/j.sleh.2018.09.006

33. Yang M, Ma N, Zhu Y, et al. The acute effects of intermittent light exposure in the evening on alertness and subsequent sleep architecture. Inter J Env Res Pub He. 2018;15:1–15. doi:10.3390/ijerph15030524

34. Borbély AA. A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol. 1982;1(3):195–204.

35. Borbély AA, Daan S, Wirz-Justice A, Deboer T. The two-process model of sleep regulation: a reappraisal. J Sleep Res. 2016;25(2):131–143. doi:10.1111/jsr.12371

36. Duffy JF, Czeisler CA. Effect of light on human circadian physiology. Sleep Med Clin. 2009;4(2):165–177. doi:10.1016/j.jsmc.2009.01.004

37. Duffy JF, Wright KP. Entrainment of the human circadian system by light. J Biol Rhythm. 2005;20(4):326–338. doi:10.1177/0748730405277983

38. Higuchi S, Lin Y, Qiu J, et al. Is the use of high correlated color temperature light at night related to delay of sleep timing in university students? A cross-country study in Japan and China. J Physiol Anthropol. 2021;40:1–8. doi:10.1186/s40101-021-00257-x

39. Zhang X, Qiao Y, Wang H, Wang J, Chen D. Lighting environmental assessment in enclosed spaces based on emotional model. Sci Total Environ. 2023;870:161933. doi:10.1016/j.scitotenv.2023.161933

40. Rekus JF. Complete Confined Spaces Handbook. 1st ed. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 2018. doi:10.1201/9781315139821

41. Health and Safety Executive. Safe Work in Confined Spaces. 1st ed. UK: HSE Books Sudbury; 1997.

42. Nieuwenhuys A, Dora J, Knufinke-Meyfroyt M, Beckers D, Rietjens G, Helmhout P. “20,000 leagues under the sea”: sleep, cognitive performance, and self-reported recovery status during a 67-day military submarine mission. Appl Ergon. 2021;91:103295. doi:10.1016/j.apergo.2020.103295

43. Trousselard M, Leger D, Van Beers P, et al. Sleeping under the ocean: despite total isolation, nuclear submariners maintain their sleep and wake patterns throughout their under sea mission. PLoS One. 2015;10:1–13. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0126721

44. Duplessis CA, Miller JC, Crepeau LJ, Osborn CM, Dyche J. Submarine watch schedules: underway evaluation of rotating (contemporary) and compressed (alternative) schedules. Undersea Hyperb Med. 2007;34:21–33.

45. Rüger M, Gordijn MCM, Beersma DGM, De Vries B, Daan S. Weak relationships between suppression of melatonin and suppression of sleepiness/fatigue in response to light exposure. J Sleep Res. 2005;14:221–227. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2005.00452.x

46. Rahman SA, Kent BA, Grant LK, et al. Effects of dynamic lighting on circadian phase, self-reported sleep and performance during a 45-day space analog mission with chronic variable sleep deficiency. J Pineal Res. 2022;73(4):e12826. doi:10.1111/jpi.12826

47. LeGates TA, Fernandez DC, Hattar S. Light as a central modulator of circadian rhythms, sleep and affect. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2014;15(7):443–454. doi:10.1038/nrn3743

48. Delorme TC, Srikanta SB, Fisk AS, et al. Chronic Exposure to Dim Light at Night or Irregular Lighting Conditions Impact Circadian Behavior, Motor Coordination, and Neuronal Morphology. Original Research. Front Neurosci. 2022;16:855154. doi:10.3389/fnins.2022.855154

49. General Equipment Department of the People’s Republic of China. GJB 523-88: Specifications for Habitability of Naval Ships. National Military Standard; 1988.

50. Commission International de l’Éclairage. Lighting of work places - part 1: indoor. In: CIE S 008/E:2001. 1st ed. Vienna: CIE; 2001.

51. Ministry of Housing and Urban-Rural Development of the People’s Republic of China. GB 50034-2013 Standard for Lighting Design of Buildings. Beijing: National Standard; 2014.

52. Bedrosian TA, Vaughn CA, Galan A, Daye G, Weil ZM, Nelson RJ. Nocturnal light exposure impairs affective responses in a wavelength-dependent manner. J Neurosci. 2013;33(32):13081–13087. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5734-12.2013

53. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39(2):175–191. doi:10.3758/BF03193146

54. von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M, Pocock SJ, Gøtzsche PC, Vandenbroucke JP. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) Statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. Inter J Surg. 2014;12(12):1495–1499. doi:10.1016/j.ijsu.2014.07.013

55. Sukegawa M, Noda A, Morishita Y, et al. Sleep and lifestyle habits in morning and evening types of human circadian rhythm. Biol Rhythm Res. 2009;40(2):121–127. doi:10.1080/09291010701794404

56. Ibáñez V, Silva J, Cauli O. A survey on sleep assessment methods. PeerJ. 2018;6:e4849. doi:10.7717/peerj.4849

57. Berka C, Levendowski DJ, Westbrook P, et al. EEG quantification of alertness: methods for early identification of individuals most susceptible to sleep deprivation. Proceedings of the SPIE Defense and Security Symposium, Biomonitoring for Physiological and Cognitive Performance during Military Operations. 2005;5797:78–89. doi:10.1117/12.597503

58. Åkerstedt T, Gillberg M. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Inter J Neurosci. 1990;52(1–2):29–37. doi:10.3109/00207459008994241

59. Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J Behav Ther Exp Psy. 1994;25(1):49–59. doi:10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9

60. Plutchik R. The nature of emotions: human emotions have deep evolutionary roots, a fact that may explain their complexity and provide tools for clinical practice. Am Sci. 2001;89(4):344–350.

61. Mehrabian A. Comparison of the PAD and PANAS as models for describing emotions and for differentiating anxiety from depression. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 1997;19(4):331–357. doi:10.1007/BF02229025

62. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54(6):1063. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

63. Lan L, Pan L, Lian Z, Huang H, Lin Y. Experimental study on thermal comfort of sleeping people at different air temperatures. Build Environ. 2014;73:24–31. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2013.11.024

64. Tsang TW, Mui KW, Wong LT. Investigation of thermal comfort in sleeping environment and its association with sleep quality. Build Environ. 2021;187:107406. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2020.107406

65. Ng CM, Kaur S. Environmental light exposure and mealtime regularity: implications for human health. Chronobiol Int. 2022;39(9):1183–1194. doi:10.1080/07420528.2022.2093212

66. Peeters ST, Smolders KCHJ, Vogels IMLC, de Kort YAW. Less is more? Effects of more vs. less electric light on alertness, mood, sleep and appraisals of light in an operational office. J Environ Psychol. 2021;74:101583. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101583

67. Green A, Cohen-Zion M, Haim A, Dagan Y. Evening light exposure to computer screens disrupts human sleep, biological rhythms, and attention abilities. Chronobiol Int. 2017;34:855–865. doi:10.1080/07420528.2017.1324878

68. Xiao J, Yu S, Chen D, et al. DHM-driven quantitative assessment model of activity posture in space-restricted accommodation cabin. Multimed Tools Appl. 2024;83(14):42063–42101. doi:10.1007/s11042-023-16842-4

69. Juliá Nehme B, Rodríguez E, Yoon SY. Spatial user experience: a multidisciplinary approach to assessing physical settings. J Interior Des. 2020;45(3):7–25. doi:10.1111/joid.12177

70. Zhang L, Bai Y, Cui X, Cao G, Li D, Yin H. Negative emotions and brain: negative emotions mediates the association between structural and functional variations in emotional-related brain regions and sleep quality. Sleep Med. 2022;94:8–16. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2022.03.023

71. Watkins MW. Exploratory factor analysis: a guide to best practice. J Black Psychol. 2018;44(3):219–246. doi:10.1177/0095798418771807

72. Zeng Y, Sun H, Yu J, Lin B. Effects of correlated color temperature of office light on subjective perception, mood and task performance. Build Environ. 2022;224:109508. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2022.109508

73. Hawes BK, Brunyé TT, Mahoney CR, Sullivan JM, Aall CD. Effects of four workplace lighting technologies on perception, cognition and affective state. Int J Ind Ergonom. 2012;42(1):122–128. doi:10.1016/j.ergon.2011.09.004

74. Islam M, Dangol R, Hyvärinen M, Bhusal P, Puolakka M, Halonen L. User acceptance studies for LED office lighting: lamp spectrum, spatial brightness and illuminance. Lighting Res Technol. 2015;47(1):54–79. doi:10.1177/1477153513514425

75. Kushida CA, Chang A, Gadkary C, Guilleminault C, Carrillo O, Dement WC. Comparison of actigraphic, polysomnographic, and subjective assessment of sleep parameters in sleep-disordered patients. Sleep Med. 2001;2(5):389–396. doi:10.1016/S1389-9457(00)00098-8

76. Sadeh A. The role and validity of actigraphy in sleep medicine: an update. Sleep Med Rev. 2011;15:259–267. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2010.10.001

77. Nie J, Zou Q, Chen L, et al. The lower correlated color temperature with higher illuminance nocturnal light environment improves cognitive performance and sleep quality. Build Environ. 2024;251:111221. doi:10.1016/j.buildenv.2024.111221

78. Höhn C, Schmid SR, Plamberger CP, et al. Preliminary results: the impact of smartphone use and short-wavelength light during the evening on circadian rhythm, sleep and alertness. Clocks Sleep. 2021;3:66–86. doi:10.3390/clockssleep3010005

79. Münch M, Scheuermaier K, Zhang R, et al. Effects on subjective and objective alertness and sleep in response to evening light exposure in older subjects. Behav Brain Res. 2011;224(2):272–278. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.05.029

80. Chellappa SL, Steiner R, Oelhafen P, Cajochen C. Sex differences in light sensitivity impact on brightness perception, vigilant attention and sleep in humans. Sci Rep. 2017;7:1–9. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-13973-1

81. Obayashi K, Yamagami Y, Kurumatani N, Saeki K. Pre-awake light exposure and sleep disturbances: findings from the HEIJO-KYO cohort. Sleep Med. 2019;54:121–125. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2018.10.027

Creative Commons License © 2024 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution - Non Commercial (unported, 3.0) License. By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.